Skip to main content

The Psychological Science of Spending Money

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Psychological Science of Money

Abstract

This chapter discusses the psychological research related to the act of spending money, with the aim of understanding the underlying psychological processes involved. To that end, the emotions involved in spending money before, during, and after the money changes hands are explored, including the role of anticipated and anticipatory emotions, different orientations to the gains and losses inherent in an act of spending, and the process of hedonic adaptation. Additionally, given how fundamental choice is to the act of spending money, factors that influence the decision-making process are discussed, including the role that comparative processes and expectations play in the process of making decisions and evaluating their outcomes. In each case, particular attention is paid to the psychological forces that influence the ultimate goal underlying any act of spending: happiness. Finally, several concrete strategies for making purchases most likely to lead to success on this goal are identified, including purchasing experiences over possessions, spending pro-socially, and making meaningful purchases.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 79.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    It is worth noting, of course, that people accumulate wealth for reasons that have nothing to do with specific planned expenditures, such as to prevent an unexpected and catastrophic life event (like an expensive health care emergency) from destroying one’s ability to meet basic needs. Indeed, the anxiety associated with debt has devastating effects on well-being (Brown, Taylor, & Price, 2005). The status that comes with wealth is also seen by some as an end in and of itself (Kasser & Ryan, 1993). While these factors undoubtedly play a role in the acquisition of wealth, because this chapter is specifically exploring the act of spending money and not its acquisition, they are better suited for discussion elsewhere.

  2. 2.

    At the extreme low end of the income spectrum, spending money might even be better thought of as intended to decrease misery rather than increase happiness (see Martin & Hill, 2012).

  3. 3.

    Those who generally feel that they spend and save appropriately are referred to as unconflicted (Rick et al., 2008).

  4. 4.

    A recent blind taste-test study found that those with some training with wine show a positive (though small) relationship between price and enjoyment, meaning that they enjoyed the more expensive wines more. Novices, however, actually showed a negative correlation; they liked the cheaper wines better (Goldstein et al., 2008).

  5. 5.

    This is in part due to companies attempting to distinguish themselves in a crowded marketplace. For any given brand, adding more options leads consumers to infer that the brand has expertise in the area, and therefore that its offerings are better (Berger, Draganska, & Simonson, 2007). This approach is, of course, less effective when everyone does it, starting the arms race that created ultra-specific options like Diet Caffeine-Free Cherry Vanilla Coke, and resulted in sagging store shelves and bewildered consumers.

  6. 6.

    A recent meta-analysis suggests that the demotivating effect of too-much-choice may be present in only certain circumstances, such as under time constraints or when the need to justify one’s choice is high (see Scheibehenne et al., 2009, 2010). This is described further below.

References

  • Ahuvia, A. (2008). If money doesn’t make us happy, why do we act as if it does? Journal of Economic Psychology, 29(4), 491–507. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2007.11.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aknin, L. B., Barrington-Leigh, C. P., Dunn, E. W., Helliwell, J. F., Burns, J., Biswas-Diener, R., et al. (2013). Prosocial spending and well-being: Cross-cultural evidence for a psychological universal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4), 635–652. doi:10.1037/a0031578.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aknin, L. B., Dunn, E. W., & Norton, M. I. (2011). Happiness runs in a circular motion: Evidence for a positive feedback loop between prosocial spending and happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, 13(2), 347–355. doi:10.1007/s10902-011-9267-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aknin, L. B., Hamlin, J. K., & Dunn, E. W. (2012). Giving leads to happiness in young children. (A. H. Kemp, Ed.). PLoS One, 7(6), 1–4. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039211.g002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aknin, L. B., Norton, M. I., & Dunn, E. W. (2009). From wealth to well-being? Money matters, but less than people think. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(6), 523–527. doi:10.1080/17439760903271421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alba, J. W., & Williams, E. F. (2013). Pleasure principles: A review of research on hedonic consumption. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 23(1), 2–18. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2012.07.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andrade, E. B., & Ariely, D. (2009). The enduring impact of transient emotions on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109(1), 1–8. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2009.02.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ariely, D. (2000). Controlling the information flow: Effects on consumers’ decision making and preferences. Journal of Consumer Research, 27(2), 233–248. doi:10.1086/314322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ariely, D., Huber, J., & Wertenbroch, K. (2005). When do losses loom larger than gains? Journal of Marketing Research, 42(2), 134–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ariely, D., Kamenica, E., & Prelec, D. (2008). Man’s search for meaning: The case of Legos. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 67(3–4), 671–677. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2008.01.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arkes, H., & Blumer, C. (1985). The psychology of sunk cost. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 35, 124–140. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(85)90049-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Avnet, T., & Higgins, E. T. (2006). How regulatory fit affects value in consumer choices and opinions. Journal of Marketing Research, 43(1), 1–10. doi:10.2307/30163364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belk, R. W. (1988). Possessions and the extended self. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 139–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, D. E. (1985). Disappointment in decision making under uncertainty. Operations Research, 33(1), 1–27. doi:10.1287/opre.33.1.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, J., Draganska, M., & Simonson, I. (2007). The influence of product variety on brand perception and choice. Marketing Science, 26(4), 460–472. doi:10.1287/mksc.1060.0253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biswas-Diener, R., & Diener, E. (2001). Making the best of a bad situation: Satisfaction in the slums of Calcutta. Social Indicators Research, 55(3), 329–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Botti, S., & McGill, A. L. (2006). When choosing is not deciding: The effect of perceived responsibility on satisfaction. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(2), 211–219. doi:10.1086/506302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyce, C. J., Wood, A. M., Banks, J., Clark, A. E., & Brown, G. D. A. (2013). Money, well-being, and loss aversion: Does an income loss have a greater effect on well-being than an equivalent income gain? Psychological Science, 24(12), 2557–2562. doi:10.1177/0956797613496436.

  • Brown, S., Taylor, K., & Price, S. W. (2005). Debt and distress: Evaluating the psychological cost of credit. Journal of Economic Psychology, 26(5), 642–663. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2005.01.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buehler, R., & McFarland, C. (2001). Intensity bias in affective forecasting: The role of temporal focus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(11), 1480–1493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caprariello, P. A., & Reis, H. T. (2013). To do, to have, or to share? Valuing experiences over material possessions depends on the involvement of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(2), 199–215. doi:10.1037/a0030953.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carmon, Z., & Ariely, D. (2000). Focusing on the forgone: How value can appear so different to buyers and sellers. The Journal of Consumer Research, 27(12), 360–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carmon, Z., Wertenbroch, K., & Zeelenberg, M. (2003). Option attachment: When deliberating makes choosing feel like losing. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(1), 15–29. doi:10.1086/374701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, T. J. (2013). The abstract and concrete nature of experiences and possessions. Unpublished manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, T. J., & Gilovich, T. (2010). The relative relativity of material and experiential purchases. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(1), 146–159. doi:10.1037/a0017145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, T. J., & Gilovich, T. (2012). I am what I do, not what I have: The differential centrality of experiential and material purchases to the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(6), 1304–1317. doi:10.1037/a0027407.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, T. J., & Gilovich, T. (2013). Getting the most for the money: The hedonic return on experiential and material purchases. In M. Tatzel (Ed.), Consumption and well-being in the material world. New York, NY: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-94-007-7368-4_3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chancellor, J., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2011). Happiness and thrift: When (spending) less is (hedonically) more. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(2), 131–138. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2011.02.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chernev, A. (2003). Product assortment and individual decision processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(1), 151–162. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.151.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). The costs and benefits of consuming. Journal of Consumer Research, 27(2), 267–272. doi:10.1086/314324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dhar, R., Nowlis, S. M., & Sherman, S. J. (1999). Comparison effects on preference construction. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(3), 293–306. doi:10.1086/209564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diehl, K., & Poynor, C. (2010). Great expectations?! Assortment size, expectations and satisfaction. Journal of Marketing Research, 47(2), 312–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2002). Will money increase subjective well-being?: A literature review and guide to needed research. Social Indicators Research, 57(2), 119–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1995). Resources, personal strivings, and subjective well-being: A nomothetic and idiographic approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(5), 926–935. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.926.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Scollon, C. N. (2006). Beyond the hedonic treadmill. American Psychologist, 61(4), 305–314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Science, 13(1), 81–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Tay, L., & Oishi, S. (2013). Rising income and the subjective well-being of nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(2), 267–276. doi:10.1037/a0030487.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, E. W., Aknin, L. B., & Norton, M. I. (2008). Spending money on others promotes happiness. Science, 319(5870), 1687–1688. doi:10.1126/science.1150952.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, E. W., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2011). If money doesn’t make you happy, then you probably aren’t spending it right. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(2), 115–125. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2011.02.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, E. W., & Norton, M. I. (2013). Happy money. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frederick, S., & Loewenstein, G. F. (1999). Hedonic adaptation. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwartz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 302–329). New York, NY: Russell Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frederick, S., Novemsky, N., Wang, J., Dhar, R., & Nowlis, S. (2009). Opportunity cost neglect. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(4), 553–561. doi:10.1086/599764.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Markman, A. B. (1994). Structural alignment in comparison: No difference without similarity. Psychological Science, 5(3), 152–158. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00652.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T., Giesler, R. B., & Morris, K. A. (1995). When comparisons arise. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(2), 227–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(3), 617–638. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.3.617.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilovich, T. (1983). Biased evaluation and persistence in gambling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(6), 1110–1126. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.6.1110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilovich, T. (1991). How we know what isn’t so: The fallibility of human reason in everyday life. New York, NY: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilovich, T., Kerr, M., & Medvec, V. H. (1993). Effect of temporal perspective on subjective confidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(4), 552–560. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.4.552.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, R., Almenberg, J., Dreber, A., Emerson, J. W., Herschkowitsch, A., & Katz, J. (2008). Do more expensive wines taste better? Evidence from a large sample of blind tastings. Journal of Wine Economics, 3(1), 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, J. G., & Broniarczyk, S. M. (2010). The slippery slope: The impact of feature alignability on search and satisfaction. Journal of Marketing Research, 47(2), 323–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, D. W., Dunning, D., & Ross, L. D. (1990). The role of construal processes in overconfident predictions about the self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(6), 1128–1139. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., & Van den Bergh, B. (2010). Going green to be seen: Status, reputation, and conspicuous conservation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(3), 392–404. doi:10.1037/a0017346.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hastie, R. (1984). Causes and effects of causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(1), 44–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haynes, G. A. (2009). Testing the boundaries of the choice overload phenomenon: The effect of number of options and time pressure on decision difficulty and satisfaction. Psychology and Marketing, 26(3), 204–212. doi:10.1002/mar.20269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, A., Heitmann, M., Morgan, R., Henneberg, S. C., & Landwehr, J. (2009). Consumer decision making and variety of offerings: The effect of attribute alignability. Psychology and Marketing, 26(4), 333–358. doi:10.1002/mar.20276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52(12), 1280–1300. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.12.1280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howell, R. T., & Hill, G. (2009). The mediators of experiential purchases: Determining the impact of psychological needs satisfaction and social comparison. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(6), 511–522. doi:10.1080/17439760903270993.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howell, R. T., Pchelin, P., & Iyer, R. (2012). The preference for experiences over possessions: Measurement and construct validation of the Experiential Buying Tendency Scale. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 7(1), 57–71. doi:10.1080/17439760.2011.626791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsee, C. K. (1996). The evaluability hypothesis: An explanation for preference reversals between joint and separate evaluations of alternatives. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67(3), 247–257. doi:10.1006/obhd.1996.0077.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsee, C. K. (1998). Less is better: When low-value options are valued more highly than high-value options. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 11(2), 107–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsee, C. K., & Leclerc, F. (1998). Will products look more attractive when presented separately or together? The Journal of Consumer Research, 25(2), 175–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsee, C. K., Loewenstein, G. F., Blount, S., & Bazerman, M. H. (1999). Preference reversals between joint and separate evaluations of options: A review and theoretical analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(5), 576–590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsee, C. K., Yang, Y., Li, N., & Shen, L. (2008). Wealth, warmth and wellbeing: Whether happiness is relative or absolute depends on whether it is about money, acquisition or consumption. Journal of Marketing Research, 46(3), 396–409. doi:10.1509/jmkr.46.3.396.

  • Hsee, C. K., & Zhang, J. (2004). Distinction bias: Misprediction and mischoice due to joint evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(5), 680–695. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.5.680.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hsee, C. K., Zhang, J., Cai, C. F., & Zhang, S. (2013). Overearning. Psychological Science, 24(6), 852–859. doi:10.1177/0956797612464785.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (2001). An influence of positive affect on decision making in complex situations: Theoretical issues with practical implications. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 11(2), 75–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (2000). When choice is demotivating: Can one desire too much of a good thing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(6), 995–1006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S. S., Wells, R. E., & Schwartz, B. (2006). Doing better but feeling worse: Looking for the “best” job undermines satisfaction. Psychological Science, 17(2), 143–150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, E. J., & Payne, J. W. (1985). Effort and accuracy in choice. Management Science, 31(4), 395–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahn, B. E., & Lehmann, D. R. (1991). Modeling choice among assortments. Journal of Retailing, 67(3), 274–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. (2004). A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method. Science, 306, 1776–1780.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American Psychologist, 39(4), 341–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kassam, K. S., Gilbert, D. T., Boston, A., & Wilson, T. D. (2008). Future anhedonia and time discounting. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(6), 1533–1537. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.07.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T. (2011). Can thrift bring well-being? A review of the research and a tentative theory. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5(11), 865–877. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00396.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T., Cohn, S., Kanner, A. D., & Ryan, R. M. (2007). Some costs of American corporate capitalism: A psychological exploration of value and goal conflicts. Psychological Inquiry, 18(1), 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: Correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(2), 410–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kihlstrom, J. F., Beer, J. S., & Klein, S. B. (2003). Self and identity as memory. In M. R. Leary & J. Tagney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (pp. 68–90). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knutson, B., Rick, S., Wimmer, G. E., Prelec, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2007). Neural predictors of purchases. Neuron, 53(1), 147–156. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.010.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koob, G. F., & Le Moal, M. (2001). Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology, 24(2), 97–129. doi:10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00195-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kumar, A., & Gilovich, T. (2013). We’ll always have Paris: Differential story utility from experiential and material purchases. Manuscript under review.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, B.-K., & Lee, W.-N. (2004). The effect of information overload on consumer choice quality in an on-line environment. Psychology and Marketing, 21(3), 159–183. doi:10.1002/mar.20000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J. S., Small, D. A., & Loewenstein, G. (2004). Heart strings and purse strings: Carryover effects of emotions on economic decisions. Psychological Science, 15(5), 337–341. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00679.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65(3), 272–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G., & Lerner, J. S. (2003). The role of affect in decision making. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 619–642). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 267–286.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H. S. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliability and construct validation. Social Indicators Research, 46(2), 137–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ma, J., & Roese, N. J. (2013). The countability effect: Comparative versus experiential reactions to reward distributions. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(6), 1219–1233. doi:10.1086/668087.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, A. B., & Medin, D. L. (1995). Similarity and alignment in choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 63(2), 117–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, K. D., & Hill, R. P. (2012). Life satisfaction, self-determination, and consumption adequacy at the bottom of the pyramid. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(6), 1155–1168. doi:10.1086/661528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mattila, A., & Wirtz, J. (2000). The role of preconsumption affect in postpurchase evaluation of services. Psychology and Marketing, 17(7), 587–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Review of General Psychology, 5(2), 100–122. doi:10.1037//I089-2680.5.2.100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mellers, B. A., & McGraw, A. P. (2001). Anticipated emotions as guides to choice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10(6), 210–214. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mellers, B., Schwartz, A., & Ritov, I. (1999). Emotion-based choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 332–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, T., Thompson, L., Peterson, E., & Cronk, R. (1997). Temporal adjustments in the evaluation of events: The “rosy view”. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(4), 421–448. doi:10.1006/jesp.1997.1333.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mochon, D., Norton, M. I., & Ariely, D. (2012). Bolstering and restoring feelings of competence via the IKEA effect. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 29(4), 363–369. doi:10.1016/j.ijresmar.2012.05.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, D. G. (2000). The funds, friends, and faith of happy people. American Psychologist, 55(1), 56–67. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.55,1.56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, L. D., & Meyvis, T. (2008). Interrupted consumption: Disrupting adaptation to hedonic experiences. Journal of Marketing Research, 45(6), 654–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newby-Clark, I. R., Ross, M., Buehler, R., Koehler, D. J., & Griffin, D. (2000). People focus on optimistic scenarios and disregard pessimistic scenarios while predicting task completion times. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 6(3), 171–182. doi:10.1037//1076-898X.6.3.171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, C. C., Schwarz, N. N., Diener, E., & Kahneman, D. D. (2003). Zeroing in on the dark side of the American dream: A closer look at the negative consequences of the goal for financial success. Psychological Science, 14(6), 531–536. doi:10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1461.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicolao, L., Irwin, J. R., & Goodman, J. K. (2009). Happiness for sale: Do experiential purchases make consumers happier than material purchases? Journal of Consumer Research, 36(2), 188–198. doi:10.1086/597049.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norton, M. I., Mochon, D., & Ariely, D. (2012). The IKEA effect: When labor leads to love. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(3), 453–460. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2011.08.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Novemsky, N., & Kahneman, D. (2005). The boundaries of loss aversion. Journal of Marketing Research, 42(2), 119–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowlis, S., Mandel, N., & McCabe, D. (2004). The effect of a delay between choice and consumption on consumption enjoyment. The Journal of Consumer Research, 31(3), 502–510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oliver, R. L. (1980). A cognitive model of the antecedents and consequences of satisfaction decisions. Journal of Marketing Research, 17, 460–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patrick, V. M., Macinnis, D. J., & Park, C. W. (2007). Not as happy as I thought I’d be? Affective misforecasting and product evaluations. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(4), 479–489. doi:10.1086/510221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pham, M. T. (1998). Representativeness, relevance, and the use of feelings in decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 25(2), 144–159. doi:10.1086/209532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, D. M., & Baumgartner, H. (2002). The role of consumption emotions in the satisfaction response. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(3), 243–252. doi:10.1207/S15327663JCP1203_06.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prelec, D., & Loewenstein, G. F. (1998). The red and the black: Mental accounting of savings and debt. Marketing Science, 17(1), 4–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prelec, D., & Simester, D. (2001). Always leave home without it: A further investigation of the credit-card effect on willingness to pay. Marketing Letters, 12(1), 5–12. doi:10.1023/A:1008196717017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quoidbach, J., Dunn, E. W., Petrides, K. V., & Mikolajczak, M. (2010). Money giveth, money taketh away: The dual effect of wealth on happiness. Psychological Science, 21(6), 759–763. doi:10.1177/0956797610371963.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reutskaja, E., & Hogarth, R. M. (2009). Satisfaction in choice as a function of the number of alternatives: When “goods satiate”. (B. Scheibehenne & P. M. Todd, Eds.). Psychology and Marketing, 26(3), 197–203. doi:10.1002/mar.20268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rick, S. I., Cryder, C. E., & Loewenstein, G. F. (2008). Tightwads and spendthrifts. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(6), 767–782. doi:10.1086/523285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, E., & Gilovich, T. (2012). Buyer’s remorse or missed opportunity? Differential regrets for material and experiential purchases. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(2), 215–223. doi:10.1037/a0024999.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, D. W., Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2012). The new stylized facts about income and subjective well-being. Emotion, 12(6), 1181–1187. doi:10.1037/a0029873.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scheibehenne, B., Greifeneder, R., & Todd, P. M. (2009). What moderates the too-much-choice effect? Psychology and Marketing, 26(3), 229–253. doi:10.1002/mar.20271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheibehenne, B., Greifeneder, R., & Todd, P. M. (2010). Can there ever be too many options? A meta‐analytic review of choice overload. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(3), 409–425. doi:10.1086/651235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schroeder, J., & Epley, N. (2013). Mistakenly seeking solitude. Manuscript under review.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. (2004). The paradox of choice: Why more is less. New York, NY: Harper Perennial.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B., Ward, A., Monterosso, J., Lyubomirsky, S., White, K., & Lehman, D. R. (2002). Maximizing versus satisficing: Happiness is a matter of choice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(5), 1178–1197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sela, A., Berger, J., & Liu, W. (2009). Variety, vice, and virtue: How assortment size influences option choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(6), 941–951. doi:10.1086/593692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sevdalis, N., & Harvey, N. (2006). Determinants of willingness to pay in separate and joint evaluations of options: Context matters. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27(3), 377–385. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2005.07.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2012). The challenge of staying happier: Testing the hedonic adaptation prevention model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(5), 670–680. doi:10.1177/0146167212436400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shiv, B., & Huber, J. (2000). The impact of anticipating satisfaction on consumer choice. The Journal of Consumer Research, 27, 202–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shugan, S. M. (1980). The cost of thinking. Journal of Consumer Research, 7(2), 99–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69(1), 99–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solnick, S., & Hemenway, D. (1998). Is more always better?: A survey on positional concerns. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 37, 373–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soman, D. (2001). Effects of payment mechanism on spending behavior: The role of rehearsal and immediacy of payments. Journal of Consumer Research, 27(4), 460–474. doi:10.1086/319621.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, E. A., Ehrlinger, J., & Eibach, R. P. (2012). Failing to commit: Maximizers avoid commitment in a way that contributes to reduced satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(1), 72–77. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.09.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tian, K. T., Bearden, W. O., & Hunter, G. L. (2001). Consumers’ need for uniqueness: Scale development and validation. The Journal of Consumer Research, 28(1), 50–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110(3), 403–421. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsiros, M., Mittal, V., & Ross, W. T., Jr. (2004). The role of attributions in customer satisfaction: A reexamination. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(2), 476–483. doi:10.1086/422124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1991). Loss aversion in riskless choice: A reference-dependent model. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 106(4), 1039–1061.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ubel, P., Loewenstein, G. F., & Jepson, C. (2005). Disability and sunshine: Can hedonic predictions be improved by drawing attention to focusing illusions or emotional adaptation? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 11(2), 111–123. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.11.2.111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Boven, L., & Ashworth, L. (2007). Looking forward, looking back: Anticipation is more evocative than retrospection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 289–300. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Boven, L., Campbell, M. C., & Gilovich, T. (2010). Stigmatizing materialism: On stereotypes and Impressions of materialistic and experiential pursuits. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(4), 551–563. doi:10.1177/0146167210362790.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Boven, L., & Gilovich, T. (2003). To do or to have? That is the question. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(6), 1193–1202. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Praag, B. M., & Frijters, P. (1999). The measurement of welfare and well-being: The Leyden approach. In Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 413–433).

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, J., Novemsky, N., & Dhar, R. (2009). Anticipating adaptation to products. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(2), 149–159. doi:10.1086/597050.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welsch, H., & Kühling, J. (2010). Pro-environmental behavior and rational consumer choice: Evidence from surveys of life satisfaction. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(3), 405–420. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2010.01.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., Centerbar, D., Kermer, D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). The pleasures of uncertainty: Prolonging positive moods in ways people do not anticipate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(1), 5–21. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting, 35, 345–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). Explaining away: A model of affective adaptation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 370–386. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00085.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., Lisle, D. J., Kraft, D., & Wetzel, C. G. (1989). Preferences as expectation-driven inferences: Effects of affective expectations on affective experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(4), 519–530. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.4.519.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, A., & Ross, M. (2003). The identity function of autobiographical memory: Time is on our side. Memory, 11(2), 137–149. doi:10.1080/741938210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Meyers, J., Gilbert, D. T., & Axsom, D. (2000). Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5), 821–836.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wirtz, D., Kruger, J., Scollon, C., & Diener, E. (2003). What to do on spring break? The role of predicted, on-line, and remembered experience in future choice. Psychological Science, 14(5), 520–524.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xiao, J. J., & Li, H. (2010). Sustainable consumption and life satisfaction. Social Indicators Research, 104(2), 323–329. doi:10.1007/s11205-010-9746-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, S., & Markman, A. B. (2001). Processing product unique features: Alignability and involvement in preference construction. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 11(1), 13–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Travis J. Carter .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Carter, T.J. (2014). The Psychological Science of Spending Money. In: Bijleveld, E., Aarts, H. (eds) The Psychological Science of Money. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-0959-9_10

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics