Skip to main content
Log in

Genetic Variation of COMT Impacts Mindfulness and Self-Reported Everyday Cognitive Failures but Not Self-Rated Attentional Control

  • ORIGINAL PAPER
  • Published:
Mindfulness Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study aimed to investigate potential associations between mindfulness and two prominent “cognitive” personality questionnaires (attentional control scale (ACS) and the cognitive failures questionnaire (CFQ)). Moreover, all participants (N = 537, n = 384 females; mean age = 23.30; SD = 5.15) provided buccal swaps for genotyping a classic polymorphism (COMT Val158Met/rs4680) known to influence dopaminergic catabolism on the biochemical level, as well as cognitive functions on a psychological level. Our results show that mindfulness is robustly linked to the ACS (positively) and the CFQ (negatively). The Met/Met genotype of the COMT polymorphism was associated with lower mindfulness (F(1, 535) = 8.27, p = .004; allele level) and higher everyday cognitive failure (F(1, 535) = 9.23, p = .003; allele level). But no significant effects of COMT Val158Met genotype could be observed for attentional control. The present findings provide evidence that the genetic COMT make up of a person could in parts explain self-reported skills in mindfulness and everyday cognitive failure. Based on the direction of the results, possible underlying factors are discussed. As such, COMT effects on an affective facet, which underlies mindfulness and everyday cognitive failures but not attentional control, may drive the effects. As still only a small part of the variance in the constructs is explained by the COMT polymorphism, potentially more genetic polymorphisms should additionally be taken into account in future studies to explain more variance. Genes involved in the dopaminergic neurotransmission seem to be an optimal starting point.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Amir, N., Beard, C., Cobb, M., & Bomyea, J. (2009). Attention modification program in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(1), 28–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Arnsten, A. F. T. (1998). Catecholamine modulation of prefrontal cortical cognitive function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(11), 436–447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., Baddeley, H. A., Bucks, R. S., & Wilcock, G. K. (2001). Attentional control in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 124, 1492–1508.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: the Kentucky inventory of mindfulness skills. Assessment, 11(3), 191–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13(1), 27–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bey, K., Montag, C., Reuter, M., Weber, B., & Markett, S. (2015). Susceptibility to everyday cognitive failure is reflected in functional network interactions in the resting brain. NeuroImage, 121, 1–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bilder, R. M., Volavka, J., Lachman, H. M., & Grace, A. A. (2004). The catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphism: relations to the tonic-phasic dopamine hypothesis and neuropsychiatric phenotypes. Neuropsychopharmacology, 29(11), 1943–1961.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., et al. (2004). Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clinical psychology. Science and Practice, 11(3), 230–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blasi, G., Mattay, V. S., Bertolino, A., Elvevåg, B., Callicott, J. H., Das, S., et al. (2005). Effect of catechol-omethyltransferase val158met genotype on attentional control. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(20), 5038–5045.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boomsma, D. I. (1998). Genetic analysis of cognitive failures (CFQ): a study of Dutch adolescent twins and their parents. European Journal of Personality, 12(5), 321–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bortz, J. (2005). Statistik für Human- und Sozialwissenschaftler. Heidelberg: SpringerMedizin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bridger, R. S., Johnsen, S. A., & Brasher, K. (2013). Psychometric properties of the cognitive failure questionnaire. Ergonomics, 56(10), 1515–1524.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E., Cooper, P. F., FitzGerald, P., & Parkes, K. R. (1982). The cognitive failures questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21, 1–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. A., Emnett, R. J., White, C. R., Yuede, C. M., Conyers, S. B., O'Malley, K. L., et al. (2010). Reduced striatal dopamine underlies the attention system dysfunction in neurofibromatosis-1 mutant mice. Human Molecular Genetics, 19(22), 4515–4528.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, J., Lipska, B. K., Halim, N., Ma, Q. D., Matsumoto, M., Melhem, S., et al. (2004). Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 75(5), 807–821.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crider, A., Blockel, L., & Solomon, P. R. (1986). A selective attention deficit in the rat following induced dopamine receptor supersensitivity. Behavioral Neuroscience, 100(3), 315–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2001). A multidisciplinary perspective on attentional control. In C. Folk & B. Gibson (Eds.), Attraction, distraction, and action (volume 133): multiple perspectives on attentional capture (pp. 325–347). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(2), 225–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, D., & Elvevåg, B. (2009). Genes, cognition and brain through a COMT lens. Neuroscience, 164(1), 72–87.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dudbridge, F. (2013). Power and predictive accuracy of polygenic risk scores. PLoS Genetics, 9(3), e1003348.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, D. R., Baer, R. A., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2009). The effects of mindfulness and selfconsciousness on persistence. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(4), 379–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, J., Wu, Y., Fossella, J. A., & Posner, M. (2001). Assessing the heritability of attentional networks. BMC Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-2-14

  • Gagne, J. R., O’Sullivan, D. L., Schmidt, N. L., Spann, C. A., & Goldmsmith, H. H. (2016). The shared etiology of attentional control and anxiety: an adolescent twin study. Journal of Research on Adolescence. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12260

  • Goldman, D., Oroszi, G., & Ducci, F. (2005). The genetics of addictions: uncovering the genes. Nature Reviews, 6(5), 521–532.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herndon, F. (2008). Testing mindfulness with perceptual and cognitive factors: external vs. internal encoding, and the cognitive failures questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(1), 32–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jurado, M. B., & Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychology Review, 17(3), 213–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kabat-Zinn, J. (2004). Wherever you go there you are: mindfulness meditation in everyday life. London: Piatkus Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keng, S.-L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of mindfulness on psychological health: a review of empirical studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(6), 1041–1056.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Klumb, P. L. (1995). Cognitive failures and performance differences: validation studies of a German version of the cognitive failures questionnaire. Ergonomics, 38(7), 1456–1467.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krabbendam, L., Isusi, P., Galdos, P., Echevarria, E., Bilbao, J. R., Martin-Pagola, A., et al. (2006). Associations between COMTVal158Met polymorphism and cognition: direct or indirect effects? European Psychiatry, 21(5), 338–342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, H. M., Papolos, D. F., Saito, T., You, Y.-M., Szumlanski, C. L., & Weinshilboum, R. M. (1996). Human catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: description of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenetics, 6(3), 243–250.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, M. A., Bishop, S. R., Segal, Z. V., Buis, T., Anderson, N. D., Carlson, L., et al. (2006). The Toronto Mindfulness Scale: development and validation. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(12), 1445–1467.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, D. A., Melchitzky, D. S., Sesack, S. R., Whitehead, R. E., Auh, S., & Sampson, A. (2001). Dopamine transporter immunoreactivity in monkey cerebral cortex: regional, laminar, and ultrastructural localization. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 432(1), 119–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288(5472), 1835–1838.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malhotra, A. K., Kestler, L. J., Mazzanti, C., Bates, J. A., Goldberg, T., & Goldman, D. (2002). A functional polymorphism in the COMT gene and performance on a test of prefrontal cognition. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(4), 652–654.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markett, S., Montag, C., & Reuter, M. (2011a). The nicotinic acetylcholine receptor gene CHRNA4 is associated with negative emotionality. Emotion, 11(2), 450–455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markett, S., Montag, C., Walter, N. T., Plieger, T., & Reuter, M. (2011b). On the molecular genetics of flexibility: the case of task switching, inhibitory control and genetic variants. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 11(4), 644–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markett, S., Montag, C., Diekmann, C., & Reuter, M. (2014). Dazed and confused: a molecular genetic approach to everyday cognitive failure. Neuroscience Letters, 566, 216–220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., Coyle, K., & Craig, A. (1990). Multiple factors of cognitive failure and their relationships with stress vulnerability. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 12(1), 49–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDonald, J. D. (2008). Measuring personality constructs: the advantages and disadvantages of self-reports, informant reports and behavioural assessments. Enquire, 1(1), 75–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mier, D., Kirsch, P., & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2010). Neural substrates of pleiotropic action of genetic variation in COMT: a meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 15(9), 918–927.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milham, M. P., Erickson, K. I., Banich, M. T., Kramer, A. F., Webb, A., Wszalek, T., & Cohen, N. J. (2002). Attentional control in the aging brain: insights from an fMRI study of the stroop task. Brain and Cognition, 49(3), 277–296.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Montag, C., & Reuter, M. (2014). Disentangling the molecular genetic basis of personality: from monoamines to neuropeptides. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 43, 228–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montag, C., Markett, S., Basten, U., Stelzel, C., Fiebach, C., Canli, T., & Reuter, M. (2010). Epistasis of the DRD2/ANKK1 Taq Ia and the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism impacts novelty seeking and harm avoidance. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(9), 1860–1867.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Montag, C., Jurkiewicz, M., & Reuter, M. (2012). The role of the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene in personality and related psychopathological disorders. CNS & Neurological Disorders - Drug Targets, 11(3), 236–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reuter, M., Schmitz, A., Corr, P., & Hennig, J. (2007). Molecular genetics support Gray’s personality theory: the interaction of COMT and DRD2 polymorphisms predicts the behavioural approach system. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 9(2), 155–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2004). Attentional control and selfregulation. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: research, theory, and applications (pp. 284–299). New Work: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmertz, S. K., Anderson, P. L., & Robins, D. L. (2009). The relation between self-report mindfulness and performance on tasks of sustained attention. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 31(1), 60–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scholes, K. E., Harrison, B. J., O’Neill, B. V., Leung, S., Croft, R. J., Pipingas, A., et al. (2007). Acute serotonin and dopamine depletion improves attentional control: findings from the Stroop task. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32(7), 1600–1610.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stelzel, C., Basten, U., Montag, C., Reuter, M., & Fiebach, C. J. (2009). Effects of dopamine related gene-gene interactions on working memory component processes. European Journal of Neuroscience, 29(5), 1056–1063.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Unsworth, N., Brewer, G. A., & Spillers, G. J. (2012a). Variation in cognitive failures: an individual differences investigation of everyday attention and memory failures. Journal of Memory and Language, 67(1), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Unsworth, N., McMillan, B. D., Brewer, G. A., & Spillers, G. J. (2012b). Everyday attention failures: an individual differences investigation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38(6), 1765–1772.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, J. J., Balint, M. G., Smolira, D. R., Kamstrup Fredericksen, L., & Madsen, S. (2009). Predicting individual differences in mindfulness: the role of trait anxiety, attachment anxiety and attentional control. Personality and Individual Differences, 46(2), 94–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waszczuk, M. A., Zavos, H. M., Antonova, E., Haworth, C. M., Plomin, R., & Eley, T. C. (2015). A multivariate twin study of trait mindfulness, depressive symptoms, and anxiety sensitivity. Depression and Anxiety, 32(4), 254–261.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wilhelm, O., Witthöfft, M., & Schipolowski, S. (2010). Self-reported cognitive failures. Journal of Individual Differences, 31(1), 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams-Gray, C. H., Hampshire, A., Barker, R. A., & Owen, A. M. (2008). Attentional control in Parkinson’s disease is dependent on COMTval158met genotype. Brain, 131(2), 397–408.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Y., Bertolino, A., Fazio, L., Blasi, G., Rampino, A., Romano, R., et al. (2007). Polymorphisms in human dopamine D2 receptor gene affect gene expression, splicing, and neuronal activity during working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(51), 20552–20557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This study was supported by the German Research Foundation as the position of Christian Montag is funded by a Heisenberg grant awarded to him by the German Research Foundation (DFG, MO 2363/3-2). CS is stipend of the German Academic Scholarship Foundation (Studienstiftung des deutschen Volkes).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

CS managed the literature searches, implemented statistical analyses, and wrote the manuscript (together with SM). SM designed the study, wrote the protocol (together with CM), wrote the manuscript (together with CS), and checked the statistical analyses. SJ did all genetic analyses and checked the statistical analyses independently. CM designed the study, wrote the protocol (together with SM), and worked over the manuscript to improve it. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript. Cornelia Sindermann and Sebastian Markett share the first authorship on this manuscript.

Cornelia Sindermann and Sebastian Markett contributed equally to this work.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cornelia Sindermann.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The study was approved by the local ethic committee at Ulm University, Ulm, Germany (https://www.uni-ulm.de/einrichtungen/ethikkommission-der-universitaet-ulm/).

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(DOC 58 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sindermann, C., Markett, S., Jung, S. et al. Genetic Variation of COMT Impacts Mindfulness and Self-Reported Everyday Cognitive Failures but Not Self-Rated Attentional Control. Mindfulness 9, 1479–1485 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-018-0893-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-018-0893-4

Keywords

Navigation