Skip to main content
Log in

Psychopathy, Executive Functions, and Neuropsychological Data: a Response to Sifferd and Hirstein

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Neuroethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Katrina Sifferd and William Hirstein, in their paper ‘On the criminal culpability of successful and unsuccessful psychopaths’, argue that neuropsychological data show that unsuccessful psychopaths have diminished mental capacities that warrant a partial or diminished responsibility defence. We respond that the currently available neuropsychological evidence does not warrant their conclusion that unsuccessful psychopaths should not be deemed completely legally responsible. Instead, we maintain that the current state of this type of research suggests that psychopaths might be suffering very specific cognitive impairments. However, the impact that these impairments might have on the specific criminal behaviours that courts have to assess is far from clear.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In fact, as we show below, Sifferd and Hirstein are mistaken in grounding the correlation between unsuccessful psychopaths and below normal EF on Ishikawa et al.’s [17] study.

  2. An explanation why people who are born without the corpus callosum do not exhibit behavioural deficits is the fact that during development their brains manage to learn how to transmit information form one hemisphere to the other by utilizing some alternative pathways [13, p. 185]. Similarly, there is evidence that core psychopathic traits are highly heritable [2, ch. 1], it could be that through development psychopaths’ brains learn to solve different EF tasks even though they show abnormal functioning in the brain areas that normally subserve EF.

  3. A reviewer of this journal has interestingly noted that in the Ishikawa et al. study, there is some reported raw data according to which there is a positive correlation between lower performance on the WCST and unsuccessful psychopathy. However, in their study, as indicated by the quote above, Ishikawa et al. do not take these raw data as relevant since these differences vanish after the IQ scores of the participants have been taken into account. In any case, Sifferd and Hirstein do not appear to rely on these raw data. Instead, it seems that they couple their incorrect reading of the main conclusion of Ishikawa et al. study with the claim that this result is robust even when participants’ IQ’s have been matched.

  4. Thus, we do not find the inconsistency in results significant because we think that there is something methodologically wrong with the studies on which Sifferd and Hirstein rely. We argue that once a plausible explanation for the mixed results is on the table, then we should see that they do not provide sufficient data for thinking that unsuccessful psychopaths, unlike successful ones, should be held less legally accountable. We thank an anonymous reviewer for nudging us to be more explicit about this point.

  5. In fact, Roald Maes and Inti Brazil [19] in their review study argue that, despite the appearances, there is no plausible evidence that there is a group of psychopaths who have superior EF.

  6. We thank an anonymous reviewer for pressing us to bring the claim in this paragraph to the foreground.

  7. In fact, the issue whether the PCL-R should be divided into two or more factors is still debated among the psychopathy researchers [37]. For an overview, see, [38]. We do not take sides on this issue. However, it has to be noted that not all researchers agree that factorization of psychopathy measures implies disunity in the construct of psychopathy. For instance, Robert Hare [22, p. 50] contends that PCL-R, despite its factorization, measures a unitary construct. On the other hand, Jalava, Griffiths, and Maraun argue that the factorization of PCL-R indicates that “there is no single, real, entity called psychopathy” [39, p. 194, see also, pp. 194–207].

  8. According to Hare [22, p. 79], Factor 1 includes: Glibness and superficial charm; Grandiose sense of self-worth; Pathological lying; Conning/manipulative; Lack of remorse or guilt; Shallow affect; Callous/lack of empathy; Failure to accept responsibility for own actions. Factor 2 includes: Need for stimulation/proneness to boredom; Parasitic life style; Poor behavioural controls; Early behavioural problems; Lack of realistic, long-term goals; Impulsivity; Irresponsibility; Juvenile delinquency; Revocation of conditional release.

  9. According to the original factor analysis, PPI’s 187 items were divided into 8 factors. These included Machiavellian Egocentricity, Social Potency, Coldheartedness, Impulsive Nonconformity, Blame Externalization, Carefree Nonplanfulness, and Stress Immunity [23, p. 44]. Later statistical analyses indicated that PPI could be divided into two factors. Social Potency, Fearlessness, and Stress Immunity form a factor that is nowadays called Fearlessness-Dominance. This includes traits such as absence of anxiety, willingness to take risks and skill at influencing others. Machiavellian Egocentricity, Impulsive Nonconformity, Blame Externalization, and Carefree Nonplanfulness form Factor 2 or Antisocial-Impulsivity traits. These include lack of concern for the social norms, attitude of indifference toward the future, and “a ruthless willingness to manipulate and take advantage of others” [23, p. 44]. These analyses also indicated that FD contains items that correlate with PCL-R’s Factor 1, while AI correlates with Factor 2 of PCL-R [21, 23].

  10. Instrumental learning tasks involve learning to connect the valenced (positive or negative) value of stimuli to an action that is appropriate for solving the task. For an overview of these tasks as they apply to psychopaths in the present context, see [40].

  11. We thank Neil Levy for pressing us to address this worry.

  12. Of course, in this occasion, we have to leave open the issue whether they have abnormal affective or other mechanisms that might undermine their responsibility, especially by affecting specific moral competencies.

  13. In this respect, the "Socratic" interviews done by philosopher Jonathan Glover and psychiatrist Gwen Adshead with the inmates of the Broadmoor Hospital are very suggestive. See, for instance, Glover’s recent book [41].

References

  1. Sifferd, K.L., and W. Hirstein. 2013. On the criminal culpability of successful and unsuccessful psychopaths. Neuroethics 6: 129–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Glenn, A.L., and A. Raine. 2014. Psychopathy: an introduction to biological findings and their implications. New York: New York University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  3. Otero, T.M., and L.A. Barker. 2014. The frontal lobes and executive functioning. In Handbook of executive functioning, ed. S. Goldstein and J.A. Naglieri, 29–44. New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  4. Schaich Borg, J., and W.P. Sinnott-Armstrong. 2013. Do psychopaths make moral judgements? In Handbook on psychopathy and law, ed. K.A. Kiehl and W.P. Sinnott-Armstrong, 107–128. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  5. Maibom, H. 2005. Moral unreason: the case of psychopathy. Mind & Language 20: 237–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Hirstein, W., and K. Sifferd. 2010. The legal self: executive processes and legal theory. Consciousness and Cognition 20: 156–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Hart, H.L.A. 1968. Punishment and responsibility: essays in the philosophy of law. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Model penal code. 1962. Philadelphia: American Law Institute.

  9. Levy, N. 2013. Psychopaths and blame: the argument from content. Philosophical Psychology 27: 351–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Kiehl, K.A. 2006. A cognitive neuroscience perspective on psychopathy: evidence for paralimibic system dysfunction. Psychiatry Research 142: 107–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Blair, J.R. 2008. The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex: functional contributions and dysfunction in psychopathy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 363: 2557–2565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Motzkin, J.C., J.P. Newman, K.A. Kiehl, and M. Koenigs. 2011. Reduced prefrontal connectivity in psychopathy. The Journal of Neuroscience 31: 17348–17357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Jeeves, M.A. 1996. Callosal agenesis. In The Blackwell dictionary of neuropsychology, ed. G.J. Beaumont, P.M. Kenealy, and M.J.C. Rogers, 182–186. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Finger, E.C., A.A. Marsh, D.G. Mitchell, M.E. Reid, C. Sims, S. Budhani, D.S. Kosson, G. Chen, K.E. Towbin, E. Leibenluft, D.S. Pine, and R.J.R. Blair. 2008. Abnormal ventromedial prefrontal cortex function in children with psychopathic traits during reversal learning. Archives of General Psychiatry 65: 586–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Gregory, S., R.J.R. Blair, D. Ffytche, A. Simmons, V. Kumari, S. Hodgins, and N. Blackwood. 2015. Punishment and psychopathy: a case-control functional MRI investigation of reinforcement learning in violent antisocial personality disordered men. Lancet Psychiatry 2: 153–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Sifferd, K.L. 2013. Translating scientific evidence into the language of the ‘folk’: executive function as capacity-responsibility. In Neuroscience and legal responsibility, ed. N.A. Vincent, 183–204. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  17. Ishikawa, S.S., A. Raine, T. Lencz, S. Bihrle, and L. Lacasse. 2001. Autonomic stress reactivity and executive functions in successful and unsuccessful criminal psychopaths from the community. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 110: 423–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Baskin-Sommers, A.R., I. Brazil, J. Ryan, N.J. Kohlenber, C.S. Neuman, and J.P. Newman. 2015. Mapping the association of global executive functioning onto diverse measures of psychopathic traits. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research and Treatment 6: 336–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Maes, J.H.R., and I.A. Brazil. 2013. No clear evidence for a positive association between the interpersonal-affective aspects of psychopathy and executive functioning. Psychiatry Research 210: 1265–1274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Koenigs, M., and J.P. Newman. 2013. The decision making impairment in psychopathy: psychological and neurobiological mechanisms. In Handbook on psychopathy and law, ed. K.A. Kiehl and W.P. Sinnott-Armstrong, 93–106. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  21. Lantrip, C., S. Towns, R.M. Roth, and P.R. Giancola. 2016. Psychopathy traits are associated with self-report rating of executive functions in the everyday life of healthy adults. Personality and Individual Differences 101: 127–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Hare, R. 2003. The psychopathy checklist-revised. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Fowler, K.A., and S.O. Lilienfeld. 2013. Alternatives to the psychopathy checklist-revised. In Handbook on psychopathy and law, ed. K.A. Kiehl and W.P. Sinnot-Armstrong, 34–57. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Ross, S.R., S.D. Benning, and Z. Adams. 2007. Symptoms of executive dysfunction are endemic to secondary psychopathy: an examination in criminal offenders and noninstitutionalized young adults. Journal of Personality Disorders 21: 384–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Mol, B., P. Van Den Bos, Y. Derks, and J. Egger. 2009. Executive functioning and the two-factor model of psychopathy: no differential relation? The International Journal of Neuroscience 119: 124–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Pera-Guardiola, V., I. Batalla, J. Bosque, D. Kosson, J. Piffare, R. Hernandez-Ribas, X. Goldberg, O. Contreras-Rodriguez, J.M. Menchon, C. Soriano-Mas, and N. Cardoner. 2016. Modulatory effects of psychopathy on Wisconsin card sorting test performance in male offenders with antisocial personality disorder. Psychiatry Research 235: 43–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. De Brito, S.A., E. Viding, V. Kumari, N. Blackwood, and S. Hodgins. 2013. Cool and hot executive function impairments in violent offenders with antisocial personality disorder with and without psychopathy. PloS One 8: 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Bagshaw, R., N.S. Gray, and R.J. Snowden. 2014. Executive function in psychopathy: the tower of London, Brixton spatial anticipation and the Hayling sentence completion tests. Psychiatry Research 220: 483–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Snowden, R.J., N.S. Gray, S. Pugh, and G. Atkinson. 2013. Executive function as a function of sub-clinical psychopathy. Personality and Individual Differences 55: 801–804.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Blair, J.R., D.G. Mitchell, and K. Blair. 2005. The psychopath: emotion and the brain. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Bechara, A., A. Damasio, and S. Anderson. 1994. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50: 7–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Brazil, I.A., J.H.R. Maes, I. Scheper, B.H. Bulten, R.P.C. Kessels, R.J.A. Verkes, and E.R. de Bruijn. 2013. Reversal deficits in individuals with psychopathy in explicit but not implicit learning conditions. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 38: E13–E20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Hamilton, R.K.B., K. Hiatt Racer, and J.P. Newman. 2015. Impaired integration in psychopathy: a unified theory of psychopathic dysfunction. Psychological Review 122: 770–791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Lösel, F., and M. Schmucker. 2004. Psychopathy, risk taking, and attention: a differentiated test of somatic marker hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 113: 522–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Glenn, A.L., A. Raine, and W.S. Laufer. 2011. Is it wrong to criminalize and punish psychopaths? Emotion Review 3: 302–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Maibom, H. 2008. The mad, the bad, and the psychopath. Neuroethics 1: 167–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Međedović, J., B. Petrović, D. Kujačić, J. Želeskov-Đorić, and M. Savić. 2015. What is the optimal number of traits to describe psychopathy? Primenjena Psihologija 8: 109–130.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Cooke, D.J., C. Michie, and Stephen D. Hart. 2006. Facets of clinical psychopathy: toward clearer measurment. In Handbook of psychopathy, ed. C.J. Patrick, 91–106. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Jalava, J., S. Griffiths, and M. Maraun. 2015. The myth of the born criminal. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

  40. Blair, J.R. 2006. Subcortical brain systems in psychopathy: the amygdala. In Handbook of psychopathy, ed. C.J. Patrick, 296–312. New York: The Guilford Press.

  41. Glover, J. 2014. Alien landscapes? Interpreting disordered minds. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Many thanks to Zdenka Brzović and Lovro Savić for reading and giving us valuable comments on different drafts of the paper. We would like to thank the two anonymous reviewers and Neil Levy, the editor of Neuroethics; their comments helped us to improve greatly this paper. In addition, we thank the organisers and participants of the following events, where parts of the paper were presented and discussed: Mente e medicina, Uniser, Pistoia, Italy 8/9/2016; Invited talk, Faculty of Media and Communications, Belgrade (Serbia), 9/6/2016; Conference: Ethical Issues: Theoretical and Applied, Bled (Slovenia) 6-10/6/2016; Invited talk, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Nijmegen (Netherlands) 12/04/2016.

The Croatian Science Foundation (HRZZ) funds our research (project CEASCRO: grants n. 8017 and n. 9522).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marko Jurjako.

Additional information

Marko Jurjako and Luca Malatesti contributed equally to this work.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jurjako, M., Malatesti, L. Psychopathy, Executive Functions, and Neuropsychological Data: a Response to Sifferd and Hirstein. Neuroethics 11, 55–65 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-016-9291-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-016-9291-6

Keywords

Navigation