Skip to main content

Agency with Impairments of Movement

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Handbook of Phenomenology and Cognitive Science

Abstract

When in 1833 Sir Charles Bell (1833) first described movement and position sense (which Sherrington was to term proprioception 60 or so years later), he immediately also saw that often we do not attend to movement.

‘… we use our limbs without being conscious, or at least, without any conception of the thousand parts which must conform to a single act … we stand by so fine an exercise of this power, and the muscles are, from habit, directed with so much precision and with an effort so slight, that we do not know how he stand. But if we attempt to walk on a narrow ledge, or stand in a situation where we are in danger of falling we become subject to apprehension; the actions of the muscles are magnified and demonstrative to the degree in which they are excited.’ Bell 1833 (italics added.)

He was aware that even similar actions can sometimes be automatic and sometimes attended to. The effects of removal of these senses of movement and position sense and of touch were investigated in 1895 by Sherrington and Mott. This was possible by sectioning the dorsal, sensory, roots of a series of monkeys.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 259.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Bell, Sir C (1833) The hand: its mechanism and vital endowments as evincing design. Bridgewater Lecture Series. London. Pickering reprinted by Pilgrim’s Press, Brentwood, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brodal A (1973) Self observations and neuro-anatomical considerations after a stroke. Brain 96:675-694

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole J (1995) Pride and a daily marathon. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole J (1998) About face. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole JD (2004a) Still lives. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole J (2004b) On the relation between sensory input and action. J Motor Behav 36(3):243-244

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole J (2007) The phenomenology of agency and intention in the face of paralysis and insentience. Phenomenol Cogn Sci 6:309-325

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole JD, Montero B (2006) Affective proprioception. Janus Head 9(2):299-320

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole J, Spalding H (2008) The invisible smile. Oxford University Press, Oxford/New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole J, Gallagher S, McNeill D (2002) Gesture following deafferentation: a phenomenologically informed experimental study. Phenomenol cogn Sci 1:49-67

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole J, Dawson A, Rawlence C, Walker L (2007) Still life in eight movements. Wellcome Trust Sciart project

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole J, Crowle S, Austwick G and Henderson-Slater D (2009) Exploratory findings in virtual induced agency for phantom limb pain. Disability & Rehabilitation, 10:846-854

    Google Scholar 

  • Desmond DM, O’Neill K, De Paor H, Mac Derby G, MacLachlan M (2006) Augmenting the reality of phantom limbs; three case studies using an augmented mirror box procedure. J Prosthet Orthot 18(3):74-79

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrer C and Frith CD (2002) Experiencing oneself vs another person as being the cause of an action. Neuroimage, 15:596-603.

    Google Scholar 

  • ffytche DH, Howard RJ (1999) The perceptual consequences of visual loss; positive pathologies of vision. Brain 122:1247-1260

    Google Scholar 

  • Fourneret P, Jeannerod M (1998) Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia 36(11):1133-1140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher S (1986) Lived body and environment. Res Phenomenol 16:139-170

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher S (2004) Neurocognitive models of schizophrenia: a phenomenological critique. Psychopathology 37:8-19 (with a response from Christopher F (2004) Comments on Shaun Gallagher. Psychopathology 37:20-22

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S (2005) How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: OUP

    Google Scholar 

  • Gandevia SC, Killian K, McKenzie DK, Crawford M, Allen GM, Gorman RB, Hales JP (1993) Respiratory sensations, cardiovascular control, kinaesthesia and transcranial stimulation during paralysis in humans. J Physiol 470:85-107

    Google Scholar 

  • Haggard P, Clark S (2003) Intentional action: conscious experience and neural prediction. Conscious Cogn 12:695-707

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haggard P, Clark S, Kalogeras J (2002) Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nat Neurosci 5(4):382-385

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haggard P and Eimer M. (1999) On the relation between brain potentials and the awareness of voluntary movements. Exp Brain Res. 126(1):128-33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haggard P and Magno E. (1999) Localising awareness of action with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Exp Brain Res. 127(1):102-7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haggard P and Libet B. (2001) Conscious intention and brain activity. Journal of Consciousness Studies, Volume 8, Number 11, pp. 47-64(18) Haggard P. Conscious intention and motor cognition. Trends in Cog Science, 9, 6, 290-295

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill J (2000) Footprints in the snow. Macmillan, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Leder D (1990) The absent body. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL

    Google Scholar 

  • Libet B, Gleason CA, Wright EW, Pearl DK (1983) Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness potential). The unconscious initiation of a free voluntary act. Brain 106:623-642

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcel A (2002) The sense of agency: awareness and ownership of action. Chapter 2, In: Roessler J, Eilan N (eds) Agency and self-awareness. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 48-93

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercier C, Reilly KT, Vargas CD, Aballea A, Sirigu A (2006) Mapping phantom movement representations in the motor cortex of amputees. Brain 129(8):2202-2210

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty M. The Primacy of Perception. Illinois: Northwestern University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Miall RC, Cole JD (2007) Evidence for stronger visuo-motor than visuo-proprioceptive conflict during mirror drawing performed by a deafferented subject and control subjects. Exp Brain Res 176(3):432-439

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mott FW, Sherrington CS (1895) Experiments upon the influence of sensory nerves upon movement and nutrition of the limbs. Proc Roy Soc 57:481-488

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulert C, Menzinger E, Leicht G, Pogarell O, Hegerl U (2005) Evidence for a close relationship between conscious effort and anterior cingulate cortex activity. Int J Psychophysiol 56(1):65-80

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy R (1987) The body silent. Henry Holt, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray CD, Pettifer S, Howard T, Patchick EL, Caillette F, Kulkarni J, Bamford C (2007) The treatment of phantom limb pain using immersive virtual reality: three case studies. Disabil Rehabil 29(18):1465-1469

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naccache L, Dehaene S, Cohen L, Habert MO, Guichart-Gomez E, Galanaud D, Willer JC (2005) Effortless control: executive attention and conscious feeling of mental effort are dissociable. Neuropsychologia. 43(9):1318-28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olausson H, Lamarre Y, Backlund H, Morin C, Wallin BG, Starck G, Ekholm S, Strigo I, Worsley K, Vallbo AB, Bushnell MC (2002) Unmyelinated tactile afferents signal touch and project to insular cortex. Nat Neurosci 5(9):900-904

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran VS, Rogers-Ramachandran D, Cobb S (1995) Touching the phantom limb. Nature 377:489-490

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reeve C (2002) Nothing is impossible; reflections on a new life. Century, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Reilly KT, Mercier C, Schieber MH, Sirigu A (2006) Persistent hand motor commands in the amputees’ brain. Brain 129(8):2211-2223

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sacks O (1981) The man who mistook his wife for a hat. Duckworth, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks O (1984) A leg to stand on. Duckworth, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherrington C (1900) Cutaneous sensation. In: Schafer EA (ed) Textbook of physiology, Pentland, Edinburgh. p 974

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsakiris M, and Haggard P (2005) Experimenting with the acting self. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(3/4):387-407

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegner D (2002) The illusion of conscious will. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA/London

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein L (1980) Remarks on the philosophy of psychology. Blackwell, Oxford, p31e

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein L (1981) Zettel, 2nd edn. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Cole, J. (2010). Agency with Impairments of Movement. In: Schmicking, D., Gallagher, S. (eds) Handbook of Phenomenology and Cognitive Science. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-2646-0_32

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics