Skip to main content

From Imitation to Reciprocation and Mutual Recognition

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Mirror Neuron Systems

Part of the book series: Contemporary Neuroscience ((CNEURO))

Abstract

Imitation and mirroring processes are necessary but not sufficient conditions for children to develop human sociality. Human sociality entails more than the equivalence and connectedness of perceptual experiences. It corresponds to the sense of a shared world made of shared values. It originates from complex ‘open’ systems of reciprocation and negotiation, not just imitation and mirroring processes that are by definition ‘closed’ systems. From this premise, we argue that if imitation and mirror processes are important foundations for sociality, human inter-subjectivity develops primarily in reciprocation, not just imitation. Imitation provides a basic sense of social connectedness and mutual acknowledgment of existing with others that are ‘like me.’ However, it does not allow for the co-construction of meanings with others. For human sociality to develop, imitation and mirroring processes need to be supplemented by an open system of reciprocation. Developmental research shows that from the second month, mirroring, imitative, and other contagious emotional responses are by-passed. Imitation gives way to first signs of reciprocation (primary intersubjectivity), joint attention to objects (secondary intersubjectivity), the emergence of values that are jointly represented and negotiated with others (tertiary intersubjectivity), and eventually the development of an ethical stance accompanying theories of mind by 4 years of age. We review this development and propose that if mirroring processes enable individuals to bridge their subjective experiences, human inter-subjectivity proper develops from reciprocal social exchanges that lead to value negotiation and mutual recognition, both cardinal trademarks of human sociality.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    This is the first definition of ‘sociability’ offered by the Unabridged Random House Dictionary (2nd Edition). ‘Sociality’ is defined as the state or quality of being sociable (third definition). This is the generic sense of these terms used here.

References

  • Agnetta, B., & Rochat, P. (2004). Imitative games by 9-, 14-, and 18-month-old infants. Infancy, 6(1), 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amsterdam, B. (1972). Mirror self-image reactions before age two. Developmental Psychobiology, 5, 297–305.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (1990). Language about me and you: Pronominal reference and the emerging concept of self. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in transition: infancy to childhood (pp. 165–182). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boggs, S. T. (1985). Speaking, r elating, and learning: A study of Hawaiian children at home and at school. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child’s talk. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Callaghan, T., Rochat, P., Lillard, A., Claux, M. L., Odden, H., Itakura, S., Tapanya, S., & Singh, S. (2005). Synchrony in the onset of mental-state reasoning: Evidence from five cultures. Psychological Science, 6(5), 378–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campos, J., & Sternberg, C. (1981). Perception, appraisal, and emotions: The onset of social referencing. In M. Lamb & L. Shjerrod (Eds.). Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations (pp. 273–314). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damon, W. (1994). Fair distribution and sharing: The development of positive justice. In B. Puka (Ed.), Fundamental research in moral development (pp. 189–254). N.Y.: Garland Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mother's voices. Science, 208, 1174–1176.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: three stages in the evolution of culture and cogntion. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finnbogason, G. (1912). The mind and the world around us, (pp. 250 and 262, cited by Hauksson, J. 2000). Acta Sociologica, 43, 307–315 .

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308(5722), 662–667.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., & Hutto, D. (2007). Understanding others through primary interaction and narrative practice. In: J. Zlatev, T. P. Racine, C. Sinha & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared Mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 17–38). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, S., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Action representation in the inferior parietal lobule. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and performance (Vol. 19, pp. 247–266). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2003). The Roots of empathy: The shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychopathology, 36, 171–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2007) Before and below “theory of mind”: embodied simulation and Philosophical Transaction of the Royal Society B, 362, 659–669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., and Watson, J. S. (1999). Early social-emotional development: Contingency perception and the social-biofeedback model. In P. Rochat (Ed.), Early social cognition (pp. 101–136). Hillsdale: Erlbaum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A., & Sripada, C. S. (2005). Simulationist models of face-based emotion recognition. Cognition, 94, 193–213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenspan, S., & Wieder, S. (2006). Engaging autism. Cambridge: Da Capo Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L. (1992). From simulation to folk psychology: The case for development. Mind and language, 7, 120–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Honneth, A. (1995). The struggle for recognition – The moral grammar of social conflicts. Cambridge (Mass): M.I.T. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. The nervous child, 2, 217–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lancy, D. (1996). Playing on the mother-ground: Cultural routines for children’s development. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M. (1992). Shame: the exposed self. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski, B. (1932). Argonauts of the Western Pacific: An account of native enterprise and adventure in the archipelagoes of Melanesian New Guinea. London: Routledge & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mauss, M. (1967). The Gift: Forms and functions of exchange in archaic societies. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by eighteen-month-old children. Developmental Psychology 31(5), 838–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). The “like me” framework for recognizing and becoming an intentional agent, Acta Psychologica, 124(1), 26–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J., & Johnson, M. H. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLERN: A two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98(2), 164–181.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulation of interactions between two-months-old and their mothers. In T. M. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 177–197). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadel, J. Carchon, I. Kervella, C., Marcelli, D., Réserbat-Plantey, D. (1999). Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science, 2(2), 164–173(10).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Odden, H., & Rochat, P. (2004). Observational learning and enculturation. Educational and Child Development, 21(2), 39–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. N.Y.: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 3(2), 131–141.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P. (2008/in press). Others in Mind–Social Origins of Self-Consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P., Neisser, U., & Marian, V. (1998). Are young infants sensitive to interpersonal contingency? Infant behavior and development, 21(2), 355–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (1999). Social cognitive development in the first year. In P. Rochat (Ed.), Early social cognition (pp. 3–34). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (2002). Who is in the mirror: Self-other discrimination in specular images by 4- and 9-month-old in fants. Child Development, 73, 35–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P., Striano, T., & Blatt, L. (2001). Differential effects of happy, neutral, and sad still-faces on 2-, 4-, and 6-month-old infants. Infant and Child Development, 11(4), 289–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P. (2001) The infant’s world. The Developing Child Series. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P. (2003). Five levels of self-awareness as they unfold early in life. Consciousness and Cognition, 12(4), 717–731.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in Thinking: Cognitive Development in Social Context. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience 27, 169–192.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P. (2006a). Humans evolved to become Homo Negotiatus ... the rest followed. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 714–715.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P. (2006b). What does it mean to be human? Anthropological Psychology, 17, 100–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P. (2008/in press). Mutual recognition as foundation of sociality and social comfort. In Striano, T., & Reid, V. (Eds.), Social cognition: Development, neuroscience and autism. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P. (2008/in press) Others in mind – Fear of rejection and the social origins of self-consciousness.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P., Dias, M. D. G., Guo, L., Broesch, T., Passos-Ferreira, C., & Winning, A. (2008/in press). Fairness in distributive justice by 3- and 5-year-olds across 7 cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P., & Passos-Ferreira, C. (2008). Homo Negotiatus. Ontogeny of the unique ways humans own, share and deal with each other. In Itakura, S., & Fujita, F. Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary and developmental views. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1995). Observing sociocultural activity on three planes: participatory appropriation, guided participation, and apprenticeship. In J. Wertsch, P. del Rio, & A. Alvarez (Eds.), Sociocultural studies of mind, (pp. 139–164). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sagi, A., & Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in the newborn. Developmental Psychology, 12, 175–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shore, B. (1982). Salailua: A samoan mystery. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & Capps, L. (1997). Children with autism – A developmental perspective. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simner, M. L. (1971). Newborn's response to the cry of another infant. Developmental Psychology, 5, 136–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1996). La Contagion des Idées. Theorie naturaliste de la culture. Paris: Odile Jacob.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Striano, T., & Rochat, P. (1999). Developmental link between dyadic and triadic social competence in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 551–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Striano, T., & Rochat, P. (2000). Emergence of selective social referencing in infancy. Infancy, 2, 253–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tarde, G. (1890/1993). Les lois de l’imitation. Paris: Editions Kimé.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H., (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and brain sciences, 16(3), 495–552.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Join attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1998). One child early talk about possession. In J. Newman, (Ed.). The linguistic of giving. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 183–229). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication (pp. 321–347). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tronick, E. Z., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., & Brazelton, T. B. (1978). The infant's response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17, 1–13.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M. (2002). Understanding the psychological world: Developing a theory of mind. In Goswami, U. (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of child cognitive development (pp. 167–187). Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, P. (1987). The development of behavioral states and the expression of emotions in early infancy. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28, 126–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

While writing this chapter, the first author was supported by a 2007 John Simon Guggenheim fellowship. Much appreciation is expressed to Britt Berg for editorial help and comments on an earlier draft.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2008 Humana Press, a part of Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Rochat, P., Passos-Ferreira, C. (2008). From Imitation to Reciprocation and Mutual Recognition. In: Pineda, J.A. (eds) Mirror Neuron Systems. Contemporary Neuroscience. Humana Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-59745-479-7_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics