Skip to main content

The Role of Mirror Neuron Dysfunction in Autism

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Mirror Neuron Systems

Part of the book series: Contemporary Neuroscience ((CNEURO))

Abstract

Autism spectrum disorders are characterized by impairments in the social, communication, and behavioral domains. Impairments in social cognition are considered a core symptom of the disorder. Dysfunction of an observation/execution matching system (the mirror neuron system) has been proposed to serve as a neural mechanism explaining the deficits in social cognition found in autism. Through direct matching of observed and executed behavior an individual can directly experience an internal representation of another’s actions, and therefore, another’s feelings, goals, and intentions. Dysfunction of this system would hinder this process. In this chapter, Bernier and Dawson review findings from behavioral research examining deficits in three areas of social cognition in individuals with autism: imitation, empathy, and theory of mind. They then discuss research implicating mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders; and explore the mirror neuron theory of autism. Autism research utilizing functional and structural brain imaging, electroencephalography, and transcranial magnetic stimulation to study the mirror neuron system is reviewed. Results from these studies suggest dysfunction of the mirror neuron system in both children and adults with autism. The mirror neuron theory of autism proposes that dysfunction of the execution/observation matching system interferes with the acquisition of internal representation of others’ observed behavior, expressions, movements, and emotions. This precludes the individual with autism having an immediate, direct experience of the other through this internal representation. Social impairments, including impairments in imitation, empathy, and theory of mind, are hypothesized to cascade from this lack of immediate, experiential understanding of others in the social world.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Aldridge, M., Stone, K., Sweeney, M., & Bower, T. (2000). Preverbal children with autism understand the intentions of others. Developmental Science, 3, 294–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Altschuler, E., Vankov, A., Hubbard, E., Robers, E., Ramachandran, V., & Pineda, J. (2000). Mu wave blocking by observation of movements and its possible use as a tool to study theory of other minds. Society of Neuroscience, 68.1 (abstract).

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., Text Revision). Washington, DC: APA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Avikainen, S., Kulomaki, T., & Hari, R. (1999). Normal movement reading in Asperger subjects. Neuroreport, 10, 3467–3470.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Avikainen, S., Wohlschlager, A., Liuhanen, S., Hanninen, R., & Hari, R. (2003). Impaired mirror-image imitation in Asperger and high-functioning autistic subjects. Current Biology, 13, 339–341.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton, P., Simonoff, E., Yuzda, E., et al. (1995). Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study. Psychological Medicine, 25, 63–77.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). Precursors to a theory of mind: Understanding attention in others. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of mind. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are children with autism superior at folk physics? New Directions in Child Development, 75, 45–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R., & Plaisted, K. (1999). Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 407–418.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 813–22.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: Aninvestigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functionin gautism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 163–175.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, J., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The Reading the Mind in the Eyes? Test-Revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241–251.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bernier, R., Dawson, G., Webb, S., & Murias, M. (2007). EEG mu rhythm and imitation impairments in individuals with autism spectrum disorder Brain & Congnition, 64, 228-237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boddaert, N., Chabane, N., Gervaise, H., Good, C., Bourgeois, M., Plumet, M., et al. (2004). Superior temporal sulcus anatomical abnormalities in childhood autism: a voxel-based morphometry MRI study. Neuroimage, 23, 364–369.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brass, M., & Heyes, C. (2005). Imitation: Is cognitive neuroscience solving the corresponding problem? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 489–495.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brune, M., & Brune-Cohrs, U. (2006). Theory of mind-evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 437–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buccino, G., Vogt, S., Ritzl, A., Fink, G., Zilles, K., Freund, H., et al. (2004). Neural circuits underlying imitation learning of hand actions: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 42, 323–334.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buitelaar, J. K., van der Wees, M., Swaab-Barneveld, H., van der Gaag, R. J. (1999). Theory of mind and emotion-recognition functioning in autistic spectrum disorders and in psychiatric control and normal children. Developmental Psychopathology, 11, 39–58.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Capps, L., Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. D. (1992). Understanding of simple and complex emotions in non-retarded children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 1169–1182.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M., Mazziotta, J., & Lenzi, G. (2003). Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems of imitation to limbic areas. Proceedings from the National Academy of Sciences in the USA, 100, 5497–5502.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1992). Understanding drawings and beliefs: a further test of the metarepresentation theory of autism: a research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 1105–1112.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). Another look at imitation in autism. Developmental Psychopathology, 6, 403–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 781–789.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1998). An experimental investigation of social-cognitive abilities in infants with autism: Clinical implications. Infant Mental Health Journal, 19, 260–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corona, R., Dissanayake, C., Arbelle, S., Wellington, P., & Sigman, M. (1998). Is affect aversive to young children with autism? Behavioral and cardiac responses to experimenter distress. Child Development, 69, 1494–1502.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dakin, S., & Frith, U. (2005). Vagaries of visual perception in autism. Neuron, 48, 597–507.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dapretto, M., Davies, M., Pfeifer, J., Scott, A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 28–30.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., & Adams, A. (1984). Imitation and social responsiveness in autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12, 209–226.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., & Lewy, A. (1989). Arousal, attention, and socioemotional impairments of individuals with autism. In G. Dawson (Ed.), Autism: Nature, diagnosis and treatment (pp. 49–74). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A., Osterling, J., & Rinaldi, J. (1998). Neuropsychological correlates of early symptoms of autism. Child Development, 69, 1276–1285.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 479–485.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., & Osterling, J. (1994). Early Intervention in Autism. In M. Guralnick’s (Ed.), The Effectiveness of early intervention (307–326). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., et al. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40, 271–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Webb, S., Carver, L., Panagiotides, H., & McPartland, J. (2004). Young children with autism show atypical brain responses to fearful versus neutral facial expressions. Developmental Science, 7, 340–359.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., & Zanolli, K. (2003). Early intervention and brain plasticity in autism. Novartis Foundation Symposium, 251, 266–274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Haan, M., Belsky, J., Reid, V., Bolein, A., & Johnson, M. (2004). Maternal personality and infants’ neural and visual responsivity to facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1209–1218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J., Chaminade, T., Grezes, J., & Meltzoff, A. (2002). A PET Exploration of the neural mechanisms involved in reciprocal imitation. Neuroimage, 15, 265–272.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • DeMeyer, M., Alpern, G., Barons, S., DeMyer, W., Churchill, D., Hingtgen, J., et al. (1972). Imitation in autistic, early schizophrenic, and non-psychotic subnormal children. Journal of Autism and Child Schizophrenia, 2, 264–287.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dissanayake, C., Sigman, M., Kasari, C., & Bagley, R. (1996). Long-term stability of individual differences in the emotional responsiveness of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 37, 461–467.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dorris, L., Espie, C., Knott, F., & Salt, J. (2004). Mind-reading difficulties in the siblings of people with Asperger's syndrome: Evidence for a genetic influence in the abnormal development of a specific cognitive domain. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 412–418.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fecteau, S., Lepage, J., & Théoret, H. (2006). Autism spectrum disorder: seeing is not understanding. Current Biology, 16, R131–R133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, P., Happé, F., Frith, U., Baker, S., Dolan, R., Frackowiak, R., et al. (1995). Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of ‘theory of mind’ in story comprehension. Cognition, 57, 109–128.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fombonne, E. (2003). Epidemiological surveys of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders: An update. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 365–382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fombonne, E. (1999). The epidemiology of autism: a review. Psychological Medicine, 29, 769–786.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C., & Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds—biological basis. Science, 286, 1692–5.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, H., & Frith, C. (2003). Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7, 77–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2006). Intentional attunement: a neurophysiological perspective on social cognition and its disruption in autism. Brain Research, 1079, 15–24.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 493–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 396–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., Hill, J., & Rutherford, M. (2006). The 'Reading the Mind in the Voice' test-revised: A study of complex emotion recognition in adults with and without autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1096–1106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, D., Baird, G., Barnett, A., Henderson, L., Huber, J., & Henderson, S. (2002). The severity and nature of motor impairment in Asperger's syndrome: a comparison with specific developmental disorder of motor function. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 655–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R., Snyder, J., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2005). Anatomical differences in the mirror neuron system and social cognition network in autism. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 1272–1282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, G., Szechtman, H., & Nahmias, C. (2003). Sex differences in regional cerebral blood flow during the processing of facial emotion. Brain and Cognition, 51, 164–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammes, J., & Langdell, T. (1981). Precursors of symbol formation and childhood autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, 331–46.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Happe F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129–154.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hertzig, M., Snow, M., & Sherman, M. (1989). Affect and cognition in autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(2), 195–199.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. (1986). The autistic child’s appraisal of expressions of emotion: A further study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 671–680.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988). Emotion recognition in autism: coordinating faces and voices. Psychological Medicine, 8, 911–923.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R., & Lee, A. (1999). Imitation and identification in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 649–659.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nature Review: Neuroscience, 7, 942–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M., Woods, R., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286(5449), 2526–2528.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M. (2005). Neural mechanisms of imitation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 632–637.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, P. L., Brunet, E., Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, J. (2006). Empathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel pain: An event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 44, 752–761.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen S. (1999). The Strange Stories Test: a replication with high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 395–406.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, V., & Prior, M. (1985). Motor imitation abilities and neurological signs in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15, 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A., Klirna, E., Bellugi, U., Grant, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Is there a social module? Language, face processing and theory of mind in individuals with Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 196–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keysers, C., Wickers, B., Gazzola, V., Anton, J., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2004). A Touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch. Neuron, 42, 335–346

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Klauck, S. (2006). Genetics of autism spectrum disorder. European Journal of Human Genetics, 14, 714–720.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Harrower, J. K., & Carter, C. M. (1999). Pivotal response intervention I: Overview of approach. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 24, 174–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S., & Perner, J. (1991). Does the autistic child have a metarepresentational deficit? Cognition, 40, 203–218.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of "theory of mind''. Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K., Tunali Kotoski, B., Pearson, D., Brelsford, K., Ortegon, J., & Chen, R. (1994). Imitation and expression of facial affect in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 433–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molnar-Szakacs, I., Iacoboni, M., Koski, L., & Mazziotta, J. (2005). Functional segregation within pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus: Evidence from fMRI studies of imitation and action observation. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 986–994.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan S., Cutrer P., Coplin J., & Rodrigue J. (1989). Do autistic children differ from retarded and normal children in Piagetian sensorimotor functioning? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 857–864.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Muthukumaraswamy, S., Johnson, B., & McNair, N. (2004). Mu rhythm modulation during observation of an object-directed grasp. Cognitive Brain Research, 19, 195–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nieminen-von Wendt, T., Metsahonkala, L., Kulomaki, T., Aalto, S., Autti, T., Vanhala, R., et al. (2003). Changes in cerebral blood flow in Asperger syndrome during theory of mind tasks presented by the auditory route. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 12, 178–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nishitani, N., Avikainen, S., & Hari, R. (2004). Abnormal imitation-related cortical activation sequences in Asperger’s Syndrome. Annals of Neurology, 55, 585–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oberman, L., Hubbard, E., McCleery, J., Altschuler, E., Ramachandran, V., & Pineda, J. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 190–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ohta, M. (1987). Cognitive disorders of infantile autism: A study employing the WISC, spatial relationships, conceptualization, and gestural imitation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 45–62.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B., & Rogers, S. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 1081–1105.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pelphrey, J., Morris, J., & McCarthy, G. (2005). Neural basis of eye gaze processing deficits in autism. Brain, 128, 1038–1048.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A., & Leekam, S. (1989). Exploration of the autistic child's theory of mind: knowledge, belief, and communication. Child Development, 60, 688–700.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pineda, J. (2005). The functional significance of mu rhythms: Translating “seeing” and “hearing” into “doing.” Brain Research Reviews, 50, 57–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pineda, J. (2006, April). Efficacy of Neurofeedback Training on Autism Spectrum Disorders (poster). Presented at Cognitive Neuroscience Society, San Francisco CA, 8–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S., & Oberman, L. M. (2006, November). Broken mirrors: A theory of autism, Scientific American.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Roeyers, H., Van Oost, P., & Bothuyne, S. (1998). Immediate imitation and joint-attention in young children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 441–450.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. (1998). Empirically supported comprehensive treatments for young children with autism. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27, 168–179.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S., & Pennington, B. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S., Bennetto, L., McEvoy, R., & Pennington, B. (1996). Imitation and pantomime in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Child Development, 67, 2060–2073.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S., Hepburn, S., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 763–781.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S., Cook, I., & Greiss-Hess, L. (2005). Mature imitation task. Unpublished coding manual, M.I.N.D. Institute, University of California – Davis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutherford, M. D., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2002). Reading the mind in the voice: A study with normal adults and adults with Asperger syndrome and high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 189–194.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people: fMRI studies of theory of mind. Neuroimage, 19, 1835–1842

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shenk, L., Hubbard, E., McCleery, J., Ramachandran, V., Pineda, J. (2004, April). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism. Cognitive Neuroscience Society Abstract.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R., & Frith, C. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not the sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., Kasari, C., Kwon, J., & Yirmiya, N. (1992). Responses to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Child Development, 63, 796–807.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. (1984). Attachment behaviors in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14(3), 231–244.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, I., & Bryson, S. (1998). Gesture imitation in autism I: Nonsymbolic postures and sequences. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 747–770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M., Rogers, S., & Dawson, G. (2008). The early start Denver model: A comprehensive early intervention approach for toddlers with autism, third edition. Austin, TX: Pro Ed Corporation, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, V., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 640–656.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W., Lemanek, K., Fishel, P., Fernandez, M., & Altemeier, W. (1990). Play and imitation skills in the diagnosis of autism in young children. Pediatrics, 64, 1688–1705.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W., Ousley, O., & Littleford, C. (1997). Motor imitation in young children with autism: What's the object? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, 475–485.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Theoret, H., Halligan, E., Kobayashi, M., Fregni, F., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Impaired motor facilitation during action observation in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Current Biology, 15, R84–R85.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Veenstra-Vanderweele, J., Christian, S., Cook, E. (2004). Autism as a paradigmatic complex genetic disorder. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics, 5, 379–405.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Veenstra-Vanderweele, J., & Cook, E. (2003). Genetics of Childhood Disorders: XLVI. Autism, Part 5: Genetics of Autism. Journal of Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 116–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogeley, K., Bussfield, P., Newen, A., Herrmann, S., Happe, F., Falkai, P., et al. (2001). Mind reading: neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspective. Neuroimage, 14, 170–181.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Waiter, G., Williams, J., Murray, A., Gilchrist, A., Perrett, D., & Whiten, A. (2004). A voxel-based investigation of brain structure in male adolescents with autistic spectrum disorder. Neuroimage, 22, 619–625.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wicker, B., Keysters, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40, 655–664.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 287–295.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J., Whiten, A., & Singh, T. (2004), A systematic review of action imitation in autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 285–299.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J., Waiter, G., Gilchrist, A., Perrett, D., Murray, A., & Whiten, A. (2006). Neural mechanisms of imitation and ‘mirror neuron’ functioning in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 44, 610–621.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Yirmiya, N., Sigman, M., Kasari, C., & Mundy, P. (1992). Empathy and cognition in high-functioning children with autism. Child Development, 63, 150–160.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zaitchik, D. (1990). When representation conflict with reality: The preschooler´s problem with false belief and false photographs. Cognition, 35, 41–68.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zilbovicius, M. Meresse, I., Chabane, N., Brunelle, F., Samson, Y., & Boddaert, N. (2006). Autism, the superior temporal sulcus and social perception. Trends in Neurosciences, 29, 359–366.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The writing of this chapter was funded by grants from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (U19HD34565, P50HD066782, and R01HD-55741) and the National Institute of Mental Health (U54MH066399). Send correspondence to Raphael Bernier, Ph.D. at University of Washington, Box 357920, Autism Center, Seattle, WA 98195.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Raphael Bernier .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2008 Humana Press, a part of Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Bernier, R., Dawson, G. (2008). The Role of Mirror Neuron Dysfunction in Autism. In: Pineda, J.A. (eds) Mirror Neuron Systems. Contemporary Neuroscience. Humana Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-59745-479-7_12

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics