Skip to main content

On the Processing of Alternatives, Exhaustification and Covert Negation: the Case of Mai

  • Chapter
Alternatives in Semantics

Abstract

In this chapter, we focus on the linguistic behavior and online processing of the Italian word mai. We are interested in mai as it exhibits typical properties of negative polarity items (NPIs) on the one hand and of negative-words (N-words) on the other. Mai exhibits sensitivity to the entailment patterns associated with the sentence in which it occurs, a feature that characterizes NPIs in general. Moreover, mai gives rise to different meanings depending on its position with respect to the verb, a characteristic of N-words. In this chapter, we propose an alternative-based analysis of mai as an NPI with the additional capacity of introducing a covert negation in certain environments in preverbal position. The main goal of this project is to investigate, through the case study of mai, the online processing of the theoretical mechanisms that we argue are involved in its semantics, such as alternatives, exhaustification and insertion of covert negation. With this goal in mind, we make explicit hypotheses on how such mechanisms are connected to online processing, and we test these hypotheses using the event-related potential (ERP) methodology. In the experiment we report below, we manipulated the presence of negation and the position of mai with respect to the verb, and we presented speakers of Italian with a set of sentences where only these two factors were varied, while their electroencephalography was recorded.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Alonso-Ovalle, L. and E. Guerzoni (2004) ‘Double Negation, Negative Concord and Metalinguistic Negation’ in M. Andronis, E. Debenport, A. Pycha and K. Yoshimura (eds) Proceedings of the Chicago Linguistics Society Meeting: the Main Session (Chicago: CLS Publications), pp. 15–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chierchia, G. (2004) ‘Scalar Implicatures, Polarity Phenomena, and the Syntax/Pragmatics Interface’ in A. Belletti (ed.) Structures and Beyond (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Chierchia, G. (2006) ‘Broaden your Views: Implicatures of Domain Widening and the “Logicality” of Language’, Linguistic Inquiry, 37(4): 535–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chierchia, G. (2011) Logic in Grammar: Polarity, Free Choice, and Intervention; to appear with Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chierchia, G., D. Fox and B. Spector (2012) ‘The Grammatical View of Scalar Implicatures and the Relationship between Semantics and Pragmatics’ in C. Maienborn, K. von Heusinger and P. Portner (eds) Semantics: an International Handbook of Natural Language Meaning, Vol. 1 (Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter).

    Google Scholar 

  • Choudhary, K., M. Schlesewsky, D. Roehm and I. Bornkessel-Schlesewsky (2009) ‘The N400 as a Correlate of Interpretively-Relevant Linguistic Rules: Evidence from Hindi’, Neuropsychologia, 47: 3012–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drenhaus, H., P. Graben, D. Saddy and S. Frisch (2006) ‘Diagnosis and Repair of Negative Polarity Constructions in the Light of Symbolic Resonance Analysis’, Brain and Language, 96: 255–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drenhaus, H., J. Blaszczak and J. Schütte (2007) ‘Some Psycholinguistic Comments on NPI Licensing’ in E. Puig-Waldmüller (ed.) Proceedings of Sinn und Bedeutung11 (Barcelona: Universitat Pompeu Fabra), pp. 180–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dwivedi, V., N. A. Phillips, M. Lague-Beauvais and S. Baum (2006) ‘An Electrophysiological Investigation of Mood’, Modal Context and Anaphora, Brain Research, 1117: 135–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, D. (2007) ‘Free Choice and the Theory of Scalar Implicatures’ in U. Sauerland and P. Stateva (eds) Presupposition and Implicature inCompositional Semantics (Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan), pp. 71–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, D. and M. Hackl (2006) ‘The Universal Density of Measurement’, Linguistics and Philosophy, 29(5): 537–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A. D., A. Hahne and D. Saddy (2002) ‘Distinct Neurophysiological Patterns Reflecting Aspects of Syntactic Complexity and Syntactic Repair’, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31: 45–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frisch, S. and M. Schlesewsky (2001) ‘The N400 Indicates Problems of Thematic Hierarchizing’, Neuroreport, 12: 3391–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gajewski, J. (2002) ‘L-Analiticity and Natural Language’, MS, University of Connecticut.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagoort, P., C. Brown and J. Groothusen (1993) ‘The Syntactic Positive Shift as an ERP-Measure of Syntactic Processing’, Language and Cognitive Processes,8: 439–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaan, E. and T. Y. Swaab (2003) ‘Repair, Revision, and Complexity in Syntactic Analysis: an Electrophysiological Differentiation’, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(1): 98–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kadmon, N. and F. Landman (1993) ‘Any’, Linguistics and Philosophy, 15: 353–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krifka, M. (1995) ‘The Semantics and Pragmatics of Polarity Items’, Linguistic Analysis, 25: 209–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M. and S. A. Hillyard (1980) ‘Reading Senseless Sentences: Brain Potentials Reflect Semantic Incongruity’, Science, 207: 203–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., C. Van Petten and R. Kluender (2006) ‘Psycholinguistics Electrified II: 1994–2005’ in M. Traxler and M. A. Gernsbacher (eds) Handbook of Psycholinguistics, 2nd edn (New York: Elsevier).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladusaw, L. A. (1979) ‘Polarity Sensitivity as Inherent Scope Relations’, PhD dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladusaw, L. A. (1992) ‘Expressing Negation’ in C. Barker and D. Dowty (eds) Proceedings of the SALT 2 (Columbus: OSU), pp. 237–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lahiri, U. (1998) ‘Focus and Negative Polarity in Hindi’, Natural Language Semantics, 6(1): 57–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laka, I. (1990) ‘Negation in Syntax: on the Nature of Functional Categories and Projections’, PhD dissertation, MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mancini, S., N. Molinaro, L. Rizzi and M. Carreiras (2011) ‘When Persons Disagree: an ERP Study of Unagreement in Spanish’, Psychophysiology, 48: 1361–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osterhout, L. and P. J. Holcomb (1992) ‘Event-Related Brain Potentials Elicited by Syntactic Anomaly’, Journal of Memoryand Language, 31: 785–804.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pablos, L., E. Shirley, K. Erdozia, I. Laka, N. Williams and D. Saddy (2011) ‘Licensing of Negative Polarity Items in Basque: an ERP study’, poster presented at the 10th International Symposium of Psycholinguistics, BCBL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panizza, D. (2012) ‘Formal Neurosemantics. Logic, Meaning and Composition in the Brain’, Journal of Neurolinguistics, 25(5): 460–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Panizza, D., G. Chierchia and C. Clifton (2009) ‘On the Role of Entailment Patterns and Scalar Implicatures in the Processing of Numerals’, Journal of Memory and Language, 61: 503–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Panizza, D., F. Vespignani, P. Zandomeneghi and R. Job (under review) ‘Never in the Wrong Place: an ERP Study on N-Words in Italian’.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinhart, T. (2006) Interface Strategies. Optimal and Costly Computations (Cambridge: MIT Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, L. (1997) ‘The Fine Structure of the Left Periphery’ in L. Haegeman (ed.) Elements of Grammar (Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers), pp. 281–337.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rooth, M. (1992) ‘A Theory of Focus Interpretation’, Natural Language Semantics, 1: 75–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saddy, D., H. Drenhaus and S. Frisch (2004) ‘Processing Polarity Items: Contrastive Licensing Costs’, Brain and Language, 90: 495–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinhauer, K., J. E. Drury, P. Portner, M. Walenski and M. T. Ullman (2010) ‘Syntax, Concepts, and Logic in the Temporal Dynamics of Language Comprehension: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials’, Neuropsychologia, 48: 1525–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Westerstâhl, D. (1985) ‘Determiners and Context Sets’ in J. van Benthem and A. ter Meulen (eds) Generalized Quantifiers inNatural Language (Dordrecht: Foris), pp. 45–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Xiang, M., B. W. Dillon and C. Phillips (2009) ‘Illusory Licensing Effects across Dependency Types: ERP Evidence’, Brain and Language, 108: 40–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zanuttini, R. (1991) ‘Syntactic Properties of Sentential Negation. A Comparative Study of Romance Languages’, doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Copyright information

© 2013 Daniele Panizza and Jacopo Romoli

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Panizza, D., Romoli, J. (2013). On the Processing of Alternatives, Exhaustification and Covert Negation: the Case of Mai. In: Fӑlӑuş, A. (eds) Alternatives in Semantics. Palgrave Studies in Pragmatics, Language and Cognition. Palgrave Macmillan, London. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781137317247_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics