Skip to main content

Gestalt-Based Linguistics and the GABEK Method:What Cognitive Science Has to Say on ‘Space Logic’ in Everyday Thought

  • Chapter
Organising Knowledge
  • 42 Accesses

Abstract

Cognitive linguistics confirms Zelger’s (1999, 2000) basic proposition about the condensation and spatial grouping of knowledge into gestalt clusters. The cognitive linguistic approach to conceptual and linguistic gestalten opens a vista on further facets of real-life cognition that might be incorporated into GABEK. Various cognitive findings point to constraints on automated gestalt building and caution us to consider top-down and bottom-up processes of thematic integration in combination. Suggestions for method development in GABEK include taking cognitive dilemmas more seriously and analysing clashing gestalten as integrated tropes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Barsalou, L. and Solomon, K. O. and Wu, L.(1999) ‘Perceptual Simulation in Conceptual Tasks’, in M. K. Hiraga, C. Sinha and S. Wilcox (eds), Cultural, Typological, and Psychological Issues in Cognitive Linguistics (Philadelphia and Amsterdam: John Benjamins), pp. 209–28.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bechtel, W. and Graham, G. (eds) (1998) A Companion to Cognitive Science (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Billig, M., Condor, S., Edwards, D., Gane, M., Middleton, D. and Radley, A. (1988) Ideological Dilemmas (London: Sage).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloch, M.(1998) How We Think They Think. Anthropological Approaches to Cognition, Memory and Literacy (Boulder, CO: Westview Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (1994) The Naturalness of Religious Ideas (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cannell, F. (1999) Power and Intimacy in the Christian Philippines (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, N. (1990) Music, Imagination and Culture (Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Deane, P. D. (1996) Grammar in Mind and Brain: Explorations in Cognitive Syntax (Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter).

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewell, R. (1994) ‘Over Again: Image Schema Transformations in Semantic Analysis’, Cognitive Linguistics, vol. 5, no. 4, pp. 351–80. Dirven, R., Frank, R. and Ilie, C. (eds) (2001) Language and Ideology, Vol. II: Descriptive Cognitive Approaches (Philadelphia and Amsterdam: John Benjamins).

    Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier, G. and Turner, M. (2002) The Way We Think. Conceptual Blending and the Mind’s Hidden Complexities (New York: Basic Books). Fillmore, C. (1977) ‘The Need for a Frame Semantics in Linguistics’, in H. Karlgren (ed.), Statistical Methods in Linguistics (Kronberg: Scriptor), pp. 5–29. Freeman, D. (1995) “Catch[ing] the Nearest Way”: Macbeth and Cognitive Metaphor’, Journal of Pragmatics, vol. 24, pp. 689–708.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, R. W. (1994) The Poetics of Mind. Figurative Thought, Language, and Understanding (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C., Pomeroy, V. and Craig, S. D.(2002) ‘Psychological and Computational Research on Theme Comprehension’, in M. Louwerse and W. van Peer (eds), Thematics. Interdisciplinary Studies (Philadelphia and Amsterdam: John Benjamins), pp. 19–34.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Grundy, P. and Jiang, Y. (2001) ‘Ideological Ground and Relevant Interpretation in a Cognitive Semantics’, in R. Dirven, B. Hawkins and E. Sandikcioglu (eds), Language and Ideology (Philadelphia and Amsterdam: John Benjamins), pp. 107–40.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Guindon, R. and Kintsch, W. (1984) ‘Priming Macropropositions: Evidence for the Primacy of Macropropositions in Memory’, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, vol. 23, pp. 508–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, B. (2001) ‘Ideology, Metaphor and Iconographic Reference’, in R. Dirven, R. Frank and C. Ilie (eds), Language and Ideology, vol. (Philadelphia and Amsterdam: John Benjamins), pp. 27–50.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. (1987) The Body in the Mind. The Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination, and Reason (Chicago, Ill.: Chicago University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimmel, M. (2002a) Metaphor, Imagery, and Culture (Vienna: University of Vienna).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimmel, M. (2002b) ‘Penetrating Into the Heart of Darkness. How Imageschematic Macrostructures Shape Plot and Megametaphor’ unpublished working paper, Vienna. Kintsch, W. (1998) Comprehension (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kovecses, Z. (1999) ‘Metaphor. Does it Constitute or Reflect Cultural Models?’, in R. Gibbs and G. Steen (eds), Metaphor in Cognitive Linguistics (Philadelphia and Amsterdam: John Benjamins), pp. 167–88.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kovecses, Z. (2000) Metaphor and Emotion. Language, Culture, Body in Human Feeling (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1987) Women, Fire and Dangerous Things. What Categories Reveal About the Mind (Chicago, Ill.: Chicago University Press).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. (1999) Philosophy in the Flesh (New York: Basic Books).

    Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. (1987) Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, vol. 1 (Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. (1990) ‘Subjectification’, Cognitive Linguistics, vol. 1, no. 1, pp. 5–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCoon, R. and Ratcliff, R. (1992) ‘Inference during Reading’, Psychological Review, vol. 99, pp. 440–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pothas, A.-M. and De Wet, A.(1999) ‘Moglichkeiten der Zusammenarbeit in einer Konfliktsituation’, in J. Zelger and M. Maier (eds), GABEK. Verarbeitung und Darstellung von Wissen (Innsbruck, Vienna and Munich: StudienVerlag), pp. 138–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, N. (1991) ‘The Cultural Basis of Metaphor’, in J. Fernandez (ed.), Beyond Metaphor. The Theory of Tropes in Anthropology (Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press), pp. 56–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1978) ‘Principles of Categorization’, in E. Rosch and B. Lloyd (eds), Cognition and Categorization (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), pp. 27–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sadoski, M. and Paivio, A. (2001) Imagery and text. A dual coding theory of reading and writing (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum).

    Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. (1991) Thinking Through Cultures (Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1996) Explaining Culture (Oxford: Blackwell).

    Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, C. and Quinn, N. (1997) A Cognitive Theory of Cultural Meaning (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweetser, E. (1987) ‘The Definition of Lie: An Examination of the Folk Models Underlying a Semantic Prototype’, in D. Holland and N. Quinn (eds), Cultural Models in Language and Thought (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), pp. 43–66.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (1988) ‘Force Dynamics in Language and Cognition’, Cognitive Science, vol. 12, pp. 49–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. R. and MacLaury, R. E. (eds) (1995) Language and the Cognitive Construal of the World (Berlin and New York: de Gruyter).

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, M. (1996) The Literary Mind (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ungerer, F. and Schmid, H.-J. (1996) An Introduction to Cognitive Linguistics (London and New York: Longman).

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijk, T. and Kintsch, W. (1983) Strategies of Discourse Comprehension (New York: Academic Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953) Philosophical Investigations (Oxford: Blackwell).

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelger, J. (1999) ‘Wissensorganisation durch sprachliche Gestaltbildung im qualitativen Verfahren GABEK’, in J. Zelger and M. Maier (eds), GABEK. Verarbeitung und Darstellung von Wissen (Innsbruck, Vienna and Munich: StudienVerlag), pp. 41–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelger, J. (2000) ‘Parallele und serielle Wissensverarbeitung: Die Simulation von Gesprachen durch GABEK’, in R. Buber and J. Zelger (eds), GABEK 11. Zur qualitativen ForschungOn Qualitative Research (Innsbruck, Vienna and Munich: StudienVerlag), pp. 31–91.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Copyright information

© 2004 Michael Kimmel

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Kimmel, M. (2004). Gestalt-Based Linguistics and the GABEK Method:What Cognitive Science Has to Say on ‘Space Logic’ in Everyday Thought. In: Gadner, J., Buber, R., Richards, L. (eds) Organising Knowledge. Palgrave Macmillan, London. https://doi.org/10.1057/9780230523111_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics