Advertisement

The Unconscious Mind

  • Michele Di Francesco
  • Massimo Marraffa
  • Alfredo Paternoster
Chapter
  • 273 Downloads

Abstract

The authors provide a critical discussion of the notion of unconscious, both in the cognitive-science sense and in the Freudian sense. The outcome of this discussion is that self-consciousness should be studied by integrating the subpersonal, bottom-up approach characteristic of cognitive sciences with recent developments in the psychodynamic framework, focusing (in particular) on the theories of cognitive-affectional relationality of the very young child: as is showed by the theories of object relations and attachment, physical contact and the construction of protective and communicative interpersonal structures constitute the infant’s primordial psychological needs, around which her mental life gradually takes form.

Keywords

Cognitive Science Mental Life Folk Psychology Extended Mind Conscious Mind 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Armstrong, D. (1968). A materialist theory of the mind. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  2. Astuti, R. (2001). Are we natural dualists? A cognitive developmental approach. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 7(3), 429–447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Balint, M. (1965). Early developmental states of the ego. Primary object love. In Primary love and psycho-analytic technique. London: Tavistock, pp. 90–108. (orig. ed. 1937).Google Scholar
  4. Barrett, J. L. (2004). Why would anyone believe in god? Plymouth: AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
  5. Bechtel, W., Abrahamsen, A., & Graham, G. (1998). The life of cognitive science. In W. Bechtel & G. Graham (Eds.), A companion to cognitive science (pp. 1–104). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  6. Bermúdez, J. L. (2005). Philosophy of psychology: A contemporary introduction. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  7. Bloom, P. (2004). Descartes’ baby. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  8. Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  9. Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  10. Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss: Sadness and depression. London: Hogarth Press and Institute of Psycho-Analysis.Google Scholar
  11. Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (2011). A cooperative species: Human reciprocity and its evolution. Princeton and Oxford: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained. The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  13. Brentano, F. (1995). Psychology from an empirical standpoint (2nd ed.). London: Routledge. (orig. ed. 1874).Google Scholar
  14. Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (2008). Internal working models in attachment relationships: Elaborating a central construct in attachment theory. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 102–130). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  15. Carpendale, J., & Lewis, C. (2006). How children develop social understanding. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  16. Castel, R. (1973). Le psychanalisme. Paris: Maspero.Google Scholar
  17. Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58, 27–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cohen, E., & Barrett, J. L. (2008). When minds migrate. Conceptualizing spirit possession. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 8(1–2), 23–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Craik, K. (1943). The nature of explanation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Cummins, R. (1997). Representations, targets and attitudes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  21. Damasio, A. (2010). Self comes to mind. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
  22. Davidson, D. (1982). Paradoxes of irrationality. In R. Wollheim & J. Hopkins (Eds.), Philosophical essays on Freud (pp. 289–305). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Davidson, D. (1985). Deception and division. In J. Elster (Ed.), The multiple self (pp. 79–92). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Dennett, D. C. (1987). The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  25. Dennett, D. C. (2009). Intentional systems theory. In B. P. McLaughlin, A. Beckermann, & S. Walter (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of philosophy of mind (pp. 339–350). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Descartes, R. (1984). Author’s replies to the first set of objections. (J. Cottingham, D. Murdoch, & R. Stootho, Eds.). The philosophical writings of Descartes (Vol. 2, pp. 74–86). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (orig. ed. 1641).Google Scholar
  27. Di Francesco, M. (2007). Extended cognition and the unity of mind: Why we are not ‘spread into the world’. In M. Marraffa, M. De Caro, & F. Ferretti (Eds.), Cartographies of the mind (pp. 213–227). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  28. Di Francesco, M., & Piredda, G. (2012). La mente estesa. Milan: Mondadori Università.Google Scholar
  29. Eagle, M. N. (2003). The postmodern turn in psychoanalysis: A critique. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 20(3), 411–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Eagle, M. N. (2011). From classical to contemporary psychoanalysis: A critique and integration. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  31. Ellenberger, H. F. (1970). The discovery of the unconscious: The history and evolution of dynamic psychiatry. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  32. Fodor, J. A. (1991, June). Too hard for our kind of mind? London Review of Books, 27, 12.Google Scholar
  33. Freud, S. (1915). Instincts and their vicissitudes. In Strachey (1956–1974), Vol. 14, pp. 117–40.Google Scholar
  34. Freud, S. (1923). The ego and the id. In Strachey (1956–1974), Vol. 19, pp. 12–66.Google Scholar
  35. Freud, S. (1929/1930). Civilization and its discontents. In Strachey (1956–1974), Vol. 21, pp. 57–146.Google Scholar
  36. Freud, S. (1933). New introductory lectures on psycho-analysis. In Strachey (1964), Vol. 22, pp. 1–267.Google Scholar
  37. Gallino, L. (2006). Dizionario di sociologia. Turin: Utet.Google Scholar
  38. Gardner, S. (1999). Psychoanalysis, contemporary views. In R. A. Wilson & F. C. Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences (pp. 683–685). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  39. Gardner, S. (2000). Psychoanalysis and the personal/sub-personal distinction. Philosophical Explorations, 3, 96–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Gennaro, R. (2012). The consciousness paradox. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  41. Gill, M. M., & Holzman, P. S. (Eds.) (1976). Psychology versus metapsychology: Essays in memory of George S. Klein. New York: International University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
  43. Goldberg, A. (1984). The tension between realism and relativism in psychoanalysis. Psychoanalysis and Contemporary Thought, 7, 367–386.Google Scholar
  44. Griffiths, P. (2004a). Instinct in the ’50s: The British reception of Konrad Lorenz’s theory of instinctive behaviour. Biology and Philosophy, 19(4), 609–631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Grünbaum, A. (1984). The foundations of psychoanalysis. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  46. Harré, R. (1986). Mind as social formation. In J. Margolis, M. Krausz, & R. M. Burien (Eds.), Rationality, relativism and the human sciences (pp. 91–106). Dordrecht: Nijhoff.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Harré, R. (1987). The social construction of selves. In K. Yardley & T. Honess (Eds.), Self and identity: Psychological perspectives (pp. 41–52). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  48. Haugeland, J. (1998). Having thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  49. Holt, R. R. (1989). Freud reappraised. A fresh look at psychoanalytic theory. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  50. Hopkins, J. (1988). Epistemology and depth psychology: Critical notes on “the foundations of psychoanalysis”. In P. Clark & C. Wright (Eds.), Mind, psychoanalysis and science (pp. 33–60). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  51. Hull, C. L. (1943). The principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  52. Hutto, D. D. (2008). Folk psychological narratives. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  53. James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover. (orig. ed. 1890).Google Scholar
  54. Jervis, G. (1989). La psicoanalisi come esercizio critico. Milan: Garzanti.Google Scholar
  55. Jervis, G. (2001). Psicologia dinamica. Bologna: il Mulino.Google Scholar
  56. Jervis, G. (2002). Un commento ai due scritti di Morris Eagle e di Robert Holt. Rassegna di Psicologia, 19(2), 45–51.Google Scholar
  57. Jervis, G. (2007). The unconscious. In M. Marraffa, M. De Caro, & F. Ferretti (Eds.), Cartographies of the mind (pp. 147–158). Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Jung, C. G. (1971). Psychological types (G. Adler & R. F. C. Hull, Eds.). The collected works of C. G. Jung (Vol. 6). Princeton: Princeton University Press. (orig. ed. 1928).Google Scholar
  59. Klein, G. S. (1976). Psychoanalytic theories: An exploration of essentials. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  60. Laplanche, J., & Pontalis, J.-B. (1973). The language of psycho-analysis. New York: W.W. Norton and Co. (orig. ed. 1967).Google Scholar
  61. Lichtenberg, J. D. (1989). Psychoanalysis and motivation. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.Google Scholar
  62. Livingstone Smith, D. (1999). Freud’s philosophy of the unconscious. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Loftus, E. F., & Ketcham, K. (1994). The myth of repressed memory. New York: St. Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
  64. Macmillan, M. (1997). Freud evaluated: The completed arc. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  65. Manson, N. C. (2000). ‘A tumbling-ground for whimsies’? The history and contemporary role of the conscious/unconscious contrast. In T. Crane & S. Patterson (Eds.), The history of the mind-body problem (pp. 148–168). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  66. Mitchell, S. A. (1988). Relational concepts in psychoanalysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  67. Moscovici, S. (2007). Psychoanalysis: Its image and its public. Cambridge: Polity Press. (orig. ed. 1961).Google Scholar
  68. Newell, A., & Simon, H. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  69. Oyama, S. (2000a). The ontogeny of information: Developmental systems and evolution. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. (orig. ed. 1985).Google Scholar
  70. Oyama, S. (2000b). Evolution’s eye: A systems view of the biology-culture divide. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Oyama, S., Griffiths, P. E., & Gray, R. D. (Eds.) (2001). Cycles of contingency: Developmental systems and evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  72. Paternoster, A. (2010). Le teorie simulative della comprensione e l’idea di cognizione incarnata. Sistemi Intelligenti, 22(1), 131–161.Google Scholar
  73. Paternoster, A. (2013). Un problema dell’inconscio cognitivo. Sistemi Intelligenti, 25(3), 469–483.Google Scholar
  74. Pears, D. (1982). Motivated irrationality, Freudian theory and cognitive dissonance. In R. Wollheim & J. Hopkins (Eds.), Philosophical essays on Freud (pp. 279–288). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  75. Peterfreund, E. (1978). Some critical comments on psychoanalytic conceptualizations of infancy. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 59, 427–441.Google Scholar
  76. Ricoeur, P. (1970). Freud and philosophy: An essay on interpretation. New Haven: Yale University Press. (orig. ed. 1965).Google Scholar
  77. Rosenthal, D. M. (1990). On being accessible to consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 621–622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Schlenker, B. R. (2012). Self-presentation. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (pp. 542–570). New York-London: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  79. Schmidt-Hellerau, C. (2005). We are driven. The Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 74(4), 989–1028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Schwalbe, M. L. (1993). Goffman against postmodernism: Emotion and the reality of the self. Symbolic Interaction, 16(4), 333–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Searle, J. (1990). Consciousness, explanatory inversion, and cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13(4), 585–596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Searle, J. (1992). The rediscovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  83. Shani, I. (2007). Consciousness and the first person. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 14, 57–91.Google Scholar
  84. Stich, S. (1983). From folk psychology to cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  85. Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  86. Tolman, E. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. The Psychological Review, 55(4), 189–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381–403). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  88. Tylor, E. B. (2010). Primitive culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (orig. ed. 1871).Google Scholar
  89. Van Gulick, R. (1995). Why the connection argument doesn’t work. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 55, 201–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Voltolini, A. (2002). Why it is hard to naturalize attitude aboutness. In W. Hinzen & H. Rott (Eds.), Belief and meaning. Essays at the interface (pp. 157–179). Hänsel-Hohenhausen: Frankfurt.Google Scholar
  91. Wilshire, B. (1969). Protophenomenology in the psychology of William James. Transactions of the Charles S. Peirce Society, 5(1), 25–43.Google Scholar
  92. Wollheim, R. (1993). Desire, belief and Professor Grünbaum’s Freud. In Id. (Ed.), The mind and its depths (pp. 91–111). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© The Author(s) 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Michele Di Francesco
    • 1
  • Massimo Marraffa
    • 2
  • Alfredo Paternoster
    • 3
  1. 1.School of Advanced Studies IUSS PaviaPaviaItaly
  2. 2.University of Roma TreRomeItaly
  3. 3.University of BergamoBergamoItaly

Personalised recommendations