Skip to main content

Avian Cognition and Consciousness—From the Perspective of Neuroscience and Behaviour

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Self, Culture and Consciousness

Abstract

Are birds conscious? Birds and primates occupy similar ecological niches, face similar challenges in foraging for food, and live in large social groups. Despite the fact that brain evolution is divergent in birds and mammals, the evolution of brain function is convergent. A number of studies on neural networks, structure, function and behaviour have demonstrated striking similarities between the overall organisation of the brain in humans, other primates and birds. Taken together, this data suggests that both cognition and consciousness may have evolved independently and in parallel across different species of birds and mammals. The present review focuses on the remarkable cognitive abilities of different species of birds such as problem-solving, tool use, mathematical abilities and self-awareness, the neural circuits underlying these behaviours and attempts to link the avian brain and behaviour to consciousness.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Aldavert-Vera, L., Costa-Miserachs, D., Divac, I., & Delius, J. D. (1999). Presumed ‘prefrontal cortex’ lesions in pigeons: Effects on visual discrimination performance. Behavioural Brain Research, 102(1), 165–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, G. E., & Crutcher, M. D. (1990). Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: Neural substrates of parallel processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 13(7), 266–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, G. E., DeLong, M. R., & Strick, P. L. (1986). Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 9(1), 357–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allman, J. M., Tetreault, N. A., Hakeem, A. Y., Manaye, K. F., Semendeferi, K., Erwin, J. M., et al. (2010). The von Economo neurons in frontoinsular and anterior cingulate cortex in great apes and humans. Brain Structure and Function, 214(5–6), 495–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allman, J. M., Watson, K. K., Tetreault, N. A., & Hakeem, A. Y. (2005). Intuition and autism: A possible role for Von Economo neurons. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9(8), 367–373. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.06.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amsterdam, B. (1972). Mirror self image reactions before age two. Developmental Psychobiology, 5(4), 297–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andén, N.-E., Carlsson, A., Dahlström, A., Fuxe, K., Hillarp, N.-Å., & Larsson, K. (1964). Demonstration and mapping out of nigro-neostriatal dopamine neurons. Life Sciences, 3(6), 523–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R., & Gallup, G. G., Jr. (2011). Which primates recognize themselves in mirrors? PLoS Biology, 9(3), e1001024.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, L. M., Pedersen, M. N., Sandberg, K., & Overgaard, M. (2016). Occipital MEG activity in the early time range (<300 ms) predicts graded changes in perceptual consciousness. Cerebral Cortex, 26(6), 2677–2688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ariëns Kappers, C. U. (1909). The phylogenesis of the paleocortex and archicortex compared with the evolution of the visual neocortex. Archives of Neurology, 4, 161–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, I. (1997). Technology and text: Glass consciousness and nineteenth-century culture. Culture, Landscape and Environment: The Linacre Lectures, 149–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atoji, Y., & Wild, J. M. (2005). Afferent and efferent connections of the dorsolateral corticoid area and a comparison with connections of the temporoparietooccipital area in the pigeon (Columba livia). Journal of Comparative Neurology, 485(2), 165–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balda, R. P., & Kamil, A. C. (1992). Long-term spatial memory in clark’s nutcracker. Nucifraga columbiana. Animal Behaviour, 44(4), 761–769. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80302-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnea-Goraly, N., Kwon, H., Menon, V., Eliez, S., Lotspeich, L., & Reiss, A. L. (2004). White matter structure in autism: Preliminary evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 55(3), 323–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartsch, C., Weiss, M., & Kipper, S. (2015). Multiple song features are related to paternal effort in common nightingales. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 15(1), 1–8. doi:10.1186/s12862-015-0390-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, F., & Eccles, J. (1992). Quantum aspects of consciousness and the role of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 89, 11357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, J. (1976). Linguistic behaviour. Hackett Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, J. (2003). Learning from six philosophers: Descartes, Spinoza, Leibniz, Locke, Berkeley. Hume: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, J., & Bennett, J. F. (1966). Kant’s analytic. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, J., & Bennett, J. F. (1988). Events and their names. Hackett Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, J. F. (1964). Rationality: An essay towards an analysis. Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bermúdez, J. L. (2003). Thinking without words (vol. 113). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (1998). What is the role of dopamine in reward: Hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Research Reviews, 28(3), 309–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bird, C. D., & Emery, N. J. (2009). Insightful problem solving and creative tool modification by captive nontool-using rooks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 106(25), 10370–10375. doi:10.1073/pnas.0901008106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (1995a). How many concepts of consciousness? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18(02), 272–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (1995b). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 18(2), 227–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. J., Flanagan, O. J., & Güzeldere, G. (1997). The nature of consciousness: Philosophical debates. Bradford Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogale, B. A., Aoyama, M., & Sugita, S. (2011). Categorical learning between ‘male’ and ‘female’ photographic human faces in jungle crows (Corvus macrorhynchos). Behavioural Processes, 86(1), 109–118. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2010.10.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boysen, S. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1989). Numerical competence in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 103(1), 23–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brainard, M. S., & Doupe, A. J. (2002). What songbirds teach us about learning. Nature, 417(6886), 351–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bugnyar, T., & Kotrschal, K. (2002). Observational learning and the raiding of food caches in ravens, Corvus corax: Is it ‘tactical’ deception? Animal Behaviour, 64(2), 185–195. doi:10.1006/anbe.2002.3056.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bugnyar, T., & Kotrschal, K. (2004). Leading a conspecific away from food in ravens (Corvus corax)? Animal Cognition, 7(2), 69–76. doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0189-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bugnyar, T., Reber, S. A., & Buckner, C. (2016). Ravens attribute visual access to unseen competetors. Nature Communications, 7, 10506. doi:10.1038/ncomms10506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, A. B., & Cotterill, R. M. (2006). Mammalian and avian neuroanatomy and the question of consciousness in birds. The Biological Bulletin, 211(2), 106–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, A. B., Manger, P. R., Lindahl, B., & Århem, P. (2005). Evolution of the neural basis of consciousness: A bird–mammal comparison. BioEssays, 27(9), 923–936.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrillo, G. D., & Doupe, A. J. (2004). Is the songbird area X striatal, pallidal, or both? An anatomical study. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 473(3), 415–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, R. L. (1988). Vertebrate palaeontology and evolution. Spektrum Akademischer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. (2000). Phenomenal consciousness: A naturalistic theory (vol. 207). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. (2006). The architecture of the mind: Massive modularity and the flexibility of thought. Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catchpole, C. K., & Slater, P. J. B. (2008). Bird song: Biological themes and variations. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cauda, F., Geminiani, G. C., & Vercelli, A. (2014). Evolutionary appearance of von Economo’s neurons in the mammalian cerebral cortex. Frontiers Human Neuroscience, 8, 104. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, L., Fang, Q., Zhang, S., Poo, M.-M., & Gong, N. (2015). Mirror-induced self-directed behaviors in rhesus monkeys after visual-somatosensory training. Current Biology, 25(2), 212–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chappell, J., & Kacelnik, A. (2002). Tool selectivity in a non-primate, the New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides). Anim Cognition, 5(2), 71–78. doi:10.1007/s10071-002-0130-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chappell, J., & Kacelnik, A. (2004). Selection of tool diameter by New Caledonian crows Corvus moneduloides. Animal Cognition, 7(2), 121–127. doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0202-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clayton, N. S., Griffiths, D. P., Emery, N. J., & Dickinson, A. (2001). Elements of episodic-like memory in animals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biological Science, 356(1413), 1483–1491. doi:10.1098/rstb.2001.0947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cornell, H. N., Marzluff, J. M., & Pecoraro, S. (2012). Social learning spreads knowledge about dangerous humans among American crows. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 279(1728), 499–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cotterill, R. M. (2001). Cooperation of the basal ganglia, cerebellum, sensory cerebrum and hippocampus: Possible implications for cognition, consciousness, intelligence and creativity. Progress in Neurobiology, 64(1), 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1990). Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness. Paper presented at the Seminars in the Neurosciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1995). Why neuroscience may be able to explain consciousness. Scientific American, 273(6), 84–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dally, J. M., Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2004). Cache protection strategies by western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica): Hiding food in the shade. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 271(Suppl 6), S387–S390. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2004.0190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dally, J. M., Clayton, N. S., & Emery, N. J. (2006a). The behaviour and evolution of cache protection and pilferage. Animal Behaviour, 72(1), 13–23. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.08.020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dally, J. M., Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2006b). Food-caching western scrub-jays keep track of who was watching when. Science, 312(5780), 1662–1665. doi:10.1126/science.1126539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C., & Barrett, P. H. (1988). On the origin of species, 1859. W. Pickering.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, D. (1984). Inquiries into truth and interpretation. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, D. (1985). Adverbs of action. In B. Vermazen & M. B. Hintikka (Eds.), Essays on Davidson: Actions and events (pp. 230–241). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daw, N. D., Kakade, S., & Dayan, P. (2002). Opponent interactions between serotonin and dopamine. Neural Networks, 15(4), 603–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Vries, T. J., & Shippenberg, T. S. (2002). Neural systems underlying opiate addiction. The Journal of Neuroscience, 22(9), 3321–3325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79(1), 1–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derégnaucourt, S., & Gahr, M. (2013). Horizontal transmission of the father’s song in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Biology Letters, 9, 20130247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Descartes, R. (1988). Descartes: Selected philosophical writings. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diekamp, B., Gagliardo, A., & Güntürkün, O. (2002). Nonspatial and subdivision-specific working memory deficits after selective lesions of the avian prefrontal cortex. The Journal of neuroscience, 22(21), 9573–9580.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dretske, F. (1995). Naturalizing the mind. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durstewitz, D., Kröner, S., & Güntürkün, O. (1999). The dopaminergic innervation of the avian telencephalon. Progress in Neurobiology, 59(2), 161–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. (1990). A unitary hypothesis of mind-brain interaction in the cerebral cortex. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 240(1299), 433–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. C. (1982). Animal consciousness and human self-consciousness. Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences, 38(12), 1384–1391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. (2003). Naturalizing consciousness: A theoretical framework. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(9), 5520–5524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edinger, L., & Rand, H. W. (1908). The relations of comparative anatomy to comparative psychology. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18(5), 437–457. doi:10.1002/cne.920180502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emery, N. J. (2006). Cognitive ornithology: The evolution of avian intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 361(1465), 23–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2001). Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays. Nature, 414(6862), 443–446. doi:10.1038/35106560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2004). The mentality of crows: Convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science, 306(5703), 1903–1907. doi:10.1126/science.1098410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emery, N. J., Dally, J. M., & Clayton, N. S. (2004). Western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica) use cognitive strategies to protect their caches from thieving conspecifics. Animal Cognition, 7(1), 37–43. doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0178-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, R., Lanza, R. P., & Skinner, B. F. (1981). Self-awareness in the pigeon. Science, 212(4495), 695–696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. (1982). The varieties of reference (vol. 137). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, S. E. (2000). Amniote evolution. In G. Bock & G. Cardew (Eds.), Evolutionary developmental biology of the cerebral cortex (pp. 109–113). Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farries, M. A., Ding, L., & Perkel, D. J. (2005). Evidence for “direct” and “indirect” pathways through the song system basal ganglia. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 484(1), 93–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farries, M. A., & Perkel, D. J. (2002). A telencephalic nucleus essential for song learning contains neurons with physiological characteristics of both striatum and globus pallidus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 22(9), 3776–3787.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuller, P., Sherman, D., Pedersen, N. P., Saper, C. B., & Lu, J. (2011). Reassessment of the structural basis of the ascending arousal system. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 519(5), 933–956.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gale, S. D., & Perkel, D. J. (2006). Physiological properties of zebra finch ventral tegmental area and substantia nigra pars compacta neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96(5), 2295–2306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallup, G. G. (1970). Chimpanzees: Self-recognition. Science, 167(3914), 86–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gennaro, R. J. (2004). Higher-order theories of consciousness: An overview. In R. J. Gennaro (ed.), Higher-order theories of consciousness. John Benjamin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gennaro, R. J. (2009). Animals, consciousness, and I-thoughts. In R. W. Lurz (ed.), The philosophy of animal minds (pp. 184–200). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Güntürkün, O., Miceli, D., & Watanabe, M. (1993). Anatomy of the avian thalamofugal pathway. Vision, brain and behavior in birds (pp. 115–135). Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gupta, S., & Sinha, A. (2014). Executive functions as a path to understanding nonhuman consciousness: Looking under the light. In S. Menon, A. Sinha, & B. V. Sreekantan (Eds.), Interdisciplinary perspectives on consciousness and the self (pp. 101–116). Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hakeem, A. Y., Sherwood, C. C., Bonar, C. J., Butti, C., Hof, P. R., & Allman, J. M. (2009). Von Economo neurons in the elephant brain. The Anatomical Record, 292(2), 242–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, B., & Güntürkün, O. (1998). Selective deficits in reversal learning after neostriatum caudolaterale lesions in pigeons: Possible behavioral equivalencies to the mammalian prefrontal system. Behavioural Brain Research, 96(1), 125–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. (2000). Wild minds: What animals really think. Henry Holt and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298(5598), 1569–1579. doi:10.1126/science.298.5598.1569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heinrich, B. (2002). Raven consciousness. In M. Bekoff, C. Allen, & G. M. Burghardt (Eds.), The cognitive animal: Empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition (pp. 47–52). MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Graham, A. (2002). Measuring executive functions in childhood: Problems and solutions? Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 7(3), 131–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. (2014). A treatise of human nature. Start Classics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Insel, T. R., & Young, L. J. (2001). The neurobiology of attachment. Nature Review of Neuroscience, 2, 129–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S. (2012). Comparative and evolutionary aspects of cognition. In P. N. Tandon, R. C. Tripathi, & N. Srinivasan (Eds.), Expanding horizons of the mind science (pp. 249–266). New York: Nova Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S., Viswanathan, S. S., & Bottjer, S. W. (1999). Development of topography within song control circuitry of zebra finches during the sensitive period for song learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19(14), 6037–6057.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, S., & Trojanowski, J. Q. (1975). Corticothalamic neurons and thalamocortical terminal fields: An investigation in rat using horseradish peroxidase and autoradiography. Brain Research, 85(3), 385–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis, E. D. (2004). Learned birdsong and the neurobiology of human language. Annal of New York Academy of Science, 1016, 749–777. doi:10.1196/annals.1298.038.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis, E. D., Güntürkün, O., Bruce, L., Csillag, A., Karten, H., & Kuenzel, W. (2005). Avian brains and a new understanding of vertebrate brain evolution. Nature Review of Neuroscience, 6(2), 151–159. doi:10.1038/nrn1606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, F., Sablan, M. M., & Bottjer, S. W. (1995). Topographic organization of a forebrain pathway involved with vocal learning in zebra finches. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 358(2), 260–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jürgens, U. (1998). Neuronal control of mammalian vocalization, with special reference to the squirrel monkey. Naturwissenschaften, 85(8), 376–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karten, H. J. (1969). The organization of the avian telencephalon and some speculations on the phylogeny of the amniote telencephalon. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 167(1), 164–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karten, H. J., Hodos, W., Nauta, W. J., & Revzin, A. M. (1973). Neural connections of the “visual wulst” of the avian telencephalon. Experimental studies in the pigeon (Columba livia) and owl (Speotyto cunicularia). Journal of Comparative Neurology, 150(3), 253–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karten, H. J., & Shimizu, T. (1989). The origins of neocortex: Connections and lamination as distinct events in evolution. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(4), 291–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, J. A., Paul, L. K., Manaye, K. F., Granstedt, A. E., Hof, P. R., Hakeem, A. Y., et al. (2008). Selective reduction of Von Economo neuron number in agenesis of the corpus callosum. Acta Neuropathologica, 116(5), 479–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Khurshid, N., Jayaprakash, N., Hameed, L. S., Mohanasundaram, S., & Iyengar, S. (2010). Opioid modulation of song in male zebra finches (Taenopygia guttata). Behavioural Brain Research, 208(2), 359–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klemen, J., & Chambers, C. D. (2012). Current perspectives and methods in studying neural mechanisms of multisensory interactions. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(1), 111–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, C., Massimini, M., Boly, M., & Tononi, G. (2016). Neural correlates of consciousness: Progress and problems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(5), 307–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Köhler, W. (1925). The mentality of apes. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koehler, O. (1950). The ability of birds to ‘count’. Bulletin of the Animal Behavioral Society, 9, 41–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koivisto, M., & Revonsuo, A. (2010). Event-related brain potential correlates of visual awareness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(6), 922–934.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kröner, S., & Güntürkün, O. (1999). Afferent and efferent connections of the Caudolateral Neostriatum in the Pigeon (Columba livia): A Retro- and Anterograde pathway tracing study. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 407, 228–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuypers, H. G. J. M. (1958). Corticobulbar connexions to the pons and lower brain stem in man. Brain, 81(3), 364–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larkum, M. (2013). A cellular mechanism for cortical associations: An organizing principle for the cerebral cortex. Trends in Neurosciences, 36(3), 141–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, W. H. (2014, 11 November). Lyrebird: The best songbird ever! [video file]. Retrieved from http://www.sciencealert.com/watch-lyrebird-perfectly-imitates-laser-gun-sounds.

  • Leutgeb, S., Husband, S., Riters, L. V., Shimizu, T., & Bingman, V. P. (1996). Telencephalic afferents to the caudolateral neostriatum of the pigeon. Brain Research, 730(1), 173–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, J. W., Ryan, S. M., Arnold, A. P., & Butcher, L. L. (1981). Evidence for a catecholaminergic projection to area X in the zebra finch. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 196(2), 347–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lissek, S., Diekamp, B., & Güntürkün, O. (2002). Impaired learning of a color reversal task after NMDA receptor blockade in the pigeon (Columbia livia) associative forebrain (Neostriatum caudolaterale). Behavioral Neuroscience, 116(4), 523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Long, L. N., & Kelley, T. D. (2010). Review of consciousness and the possibility of conscious robots. Journal of Aerospace Computing, Information, and Communication, 7(2), 68–84. doi:10.2514/1.46188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, P., Panksepp, J., Reiss, D., Edelman, D., Swinderen, B. V., & Koch, C. (2012, 7 July). The Cambridge declaration on consciousness. http://fcmconference.org/img/CambridgeDeclarationOnConsciousness.pdf. Accessed December 18, 2016.

  • Luo, M., Ding, L., & Perkel, D. J. (2001). An avian basal ganglia pathway essential for vocal learning forms a closed topographic loop. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21(17), 6836–6845.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lurz, R. W. (2004). In search of the metaphor of the mind: A critical review of Baars’ in the theater of consciousness. Philosophical Psychology, 17(2), 297–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lurz, R. W. (2006). Conscious beliefs and desires: A same-order approach. In U. Kriegel & K. Williford (Eds.), Self-representational approaches to consciousness. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lurz, R. W. (2009). Animal minds. Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lutkenhoff, E. S., Chiang, J., Tshibanda, L., Kamau, E., Kirsch, M., Pickard, J. D., et al. (2015). Thalamic and extrathalamic mechanisms of consciousness after severe brain injury. Annals of Neurology, 78(1), 68–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lycan, W. G. (1996). Consciousness and experience (vol. 72). MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynd-Balta, E., & Haber, S. (1994). The organization of midbrain projections to the striatum in the primate: Sensorimotor-related striatum versus ventral striatum. Neuroscience, 59(3), 625–640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macphail, E. M. (1982). Brain and intelligence in vertebrates. Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malcolm, N. (1972). Thoughtless Brutes. Proceedings and Addresses of the American Philosophical Association, 46, 5–20. doi:10.2307/3129585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcel, A. J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 197–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marler, P., & Slabbekoorn, H. W. (2004). Nature’s music: The science of birdsong. Elsevier Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCasland, J. S. (1987). Neuronal control of bird song production. The Journal of Neuroscience, 7(1), 23–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medina, F., Taylor, A., Hunt, G., & Gray, R. D. (2011). New Caledonian crows’ responses to mirrors. Animal Behaviour, 82(5), 981–993.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melloni, L., Schwiedrzik, C. M., Müller, N., Rodriguez, E., & Singer, W. (2011). Expectations change the signatures and timing of electrophysiological correlates of perceptual awareness. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(4), 1386–1396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metzger, M., Jiang, S., & Braun, K. (1998). Organization of the dorsocaudal neostriatal complex: A retrograde and anterograde tracing study in the domestic chick with special emphasis on pathways relevant to imprinting. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 395(3), 380–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogensen, J., & Divac, I. (1993). Behavioural effects of ablation of the pigeon-equivalent of the mammalian prefrontal cortex. Behavioural Brain Research, 55(1), 101–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montagnese, C. M., Mezey, S. E., & Csillag, A. (2003). Efferent connections of the dorsomedial thalamic nuclei of the domestic chick (Gallus domesticus). Journal of Comparative Neurology, 459(3), 301–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? The Philosophical Review, 83(4), 435–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakajimaa, S., Gerretsena, P., Takeuchic, H., Caravaggioa, F., Chowb, T., Follc, B. L., et al. (2013). The potential role of dopamine D3 receptor neurotransmission in cognition. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 23(8), 799–813. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.05.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nauta, W. J., & Karten, H. J. (1970). A general profile of the vertebrate brain, with sidelights on the ancestry of cerebral cortex. In Neuroanatomy (pp. 520–539). Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nauta, W. J., & Kuypers, H. G. M. (1958). Some ascending pathways in the brain stem reticular formation. In J. H. Herbert, L. D. Proctor, R. S. Knighton, W. C. Noshay, & R. T. Costello (Eds.), Reticular formation of the brain (pp. 3–30). Oxford: Little Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nottebohm, F., Paton, J. A., & Kelley, D. B. (1982). Connections of vocal control nuclei in the canary telencephalon. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 207(4), 344–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nottebohm, F., Stokes, T. M., & Leonard, C. M. (1976). Central control of song in the Canary, Serinus Canarius. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 165(4), 457–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oizumi, M., Albantakis, L., & Tononi, G. (2014). From the phenomenology to the mechanisms of consciousness: Integrated information theory 3.0. PLoS Computational Biology, 10(5), e1003588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parent, A., & Hazrati, L.-N. (1995). Functional anatomy of the basal ganglia. I. The cortico-basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical loop. Brain Research Reviews, 20(1), 91–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pepperberg, I. M. (1981). Functional vocalizations by an African Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 55(2), 139–160. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1981.tb01265.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pepperberg, I. M. (1987). Acquisition of the same/different concept by an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): Learning with respect to categories of color, shape, and material. Animal Learning and Behavior, 15(4), 423–432. doi:10.3758/bf03205051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pepperberg, I. M. (1997). Social influences on the acquisition of human-based codes in parrots and nonhuman primates. In C. T. Snowdon & M. Hausberger (Eds.), Social influences on vocal development (pp. 157–177). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pepperberg, I. M. (1999). The Alex studies. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pepperberg, I. M. (2006). Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus) numerical abilities: Addition and further experiments on a zero-like concept. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(1), 1–11. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.120.1.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pepperberg, I. M. (2007). Grey parrots do not always ‘parrot’: The roles of imitation and phonological awareness in the creation of new labels from existing vocalizations. Language Sciences, 29(1), 1–13. doi:10.1016/j.langsci.2005.12.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pepperberg, I. M., Brese, K. J., & Harris, B. J. (1991). Solitary sound play during acquisition of English vocalizations by an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): Possible parallels with children’s monologue speech. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12(02), 151–178. doi:10.1017/S0142716400009127.

  • Pepperberg, I. M., Sandefer, R. M., Noel, D. A., & Ellsworth, C. P. (2000). Vocal learning in the Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): Effects of species identity and number of trainers. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114(4), 371–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pepperberg, I. M., Willner, M. R., & Gravitz, L. B. (1997). Development of Piagetian object permanence in a grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111(1), 63–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). La naissance de l’intelligence chez l’enfant. International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pitts, M. A., Metzler, S., & Hillyard, S. A. (2014). Isolating neural correlates of conscious perception from neural correlates of reporting one’s perception. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1078. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01078.

  • Plotnik, J. M., De Waal, F. B., & Reiss, D. (2006). Self-recognition in an Asian elephant. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(45), 17053–17057.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollok, B., Prior, H., & Güntürkün, O. (2000). Development of object permanence in food-storing magpies (Pica pica). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114(2), 148–157. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.114.2.148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1998). Attention, self–regulation and consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 353(1377), 1915–1927.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prior, H., Schwarz, A., & Güntürkün, O. (2008). Mirror-induced behavior in the magpie (Pica pica): Evidence of self-recognition. PLoS Biology, 6(8), e202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putland, D. A., Nicholls, J. A., Noad, M. J., & Goldizen, A. W. (2006). Imitating the neighbours: Vocal dialect matching in a mimic-model system. Biology Letters, 2(3), 367–370. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2006.0502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radner, D., & Radner, M. (1989). Animal consciousness. Prometheus Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rattenborg, N., Martinez, D., Roth, T. C., & Pravosudov, V. V. (2011). Hippocampal memory consolidation during sleep: A comparison of mammals and birds. Biological Reviews Cambridge Philosophical Society, 86(3), 658–691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rees, G., Kreiman, G., & Koch, C. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(4), 261–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiner, A. (1986). The co-occurrence of substance P-like immunoreactivity and dynorphin-like immunoreactivity in striatopallidal and striatonigral projection neurons in birds and reptiles. Brain Research, 371(1), 155–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiner, A., Laverghetta, A. V., Meade, C. A., Cuthbertson, S. L., & Bottjer, S. W. (2004a). An immunohistochemical and pathway tracing study of the striatopallidal organization of area X in the male zebra finch. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 469(2), 239–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiner, A., Perkel, D. J., Bruce, L. L., Butler, A. B., Csillag, A., Kuenzel, W., et al. (2004b). Revised nomenclature for avian telencephalon and some related brainstem nuclei. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 473(3), 377–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiss, D., & Marino, L. (2001). Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A case of cognitive convergence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 98(10), 5937–5942.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, S., Sandstrom, S. M., Denenberg, V. H., & Palmiter, R. D. (2005). Distinguishing whether dopamine regulates liking, wanting, and/or learning about rewards. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119(1), 5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, J., & Colombo, M. (2005). Neural correlates of executive control in the avian brain. PLoS Biology, 3(6), e190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, D. M. (1986). Two concepts of consciousness. Philosophical Studies, 49, 329–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sandberg, K., Del Pin, S. H., Bibby, B. M., & Overgaard, M. (2014). Evidence of weak conscious experiences in the exclusion task. Frontiers in Psychology, 23(5), 1080. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01080. eCollection 2014.

  • Sasaki, A., Sotnikova, T. D., Gainetdinov, R. R., & Jarvis, E. D. (2006). Social context-dependent singing-regulated dopamine. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(35), 9010–9014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Shanker, S. G., & Taylor, T. J. (1998). Apes, language, and the human mind. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiff, N. D., Giacino, J., Kalmar, K., Victor, J., Baker, K., Gerber, M., et al. (2007). Behavioural improvements with thalamic stimulation after severe traumatic brain injury. Nature, 448(7153), 600–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seager, W., & Bourget, D. (2007). Representationalism about consciousness. In The Blackwell companion to consciousness (pp. 261–276).

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. (1994). Animal Minds. Midwest Studies IN Philosophy, 19(1), 206–219. doi:10.1111/j.1475-4975.1994.tb00286.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. (2003). Rationality in action. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sergent, C., Baillet, S., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Timing of the brain events underlying access to consciousness during the attentional blink. Nature Neuroscience, 8(10), 1391–1400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siclari, F., Bernardi, G., Riedner, B. A., LaRocque, J. J., Benca, R. M., & Tononi, G. (2014). Two distinct synchronization processes in the transition to sleep: A high-density electroencephalographic study. Sleep, 37(10), 1621.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, A. (1999). Almost minds? The search for consciousness in nonhuman primates. Scientific and Philosophical Studies on Consciousness, 149–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D. T., & Alexander, M. P. (2000). Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A conceptual view. Psychological Research, 63(3–4), 289–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf, T., & Butler, D. L. (2013). The nature of visual self-recognition. Trends in Cognitive Science, 17(3), 121–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf, T., & Collier-Baker, E. (2009). The evolution of primate visual self-recognition: Evidence of absence in lesser apes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 276(1662), 1671–1677.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, A. H. [BBC]. (2014, 4 February). Are crows the ultimate problem solvers?—Inside the animal mind: episode 2. BBC2 [video file]. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=AVaITA7eBZE. Accessed December 12, 2016.

  • Taylor, A. H., Knaebe, B., & Gray, R. D. (2012). An end to insight? New Caledonian crows can spontaneously solve problems without planning their actions. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 279(1749), 4977–4981.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tetzlaff, M., & Rey, G. (2009). Systematicity and intentional realism in honeybee navigation. In R. W. Lurz. The philosophy of animal minds. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorpe, W. H. (1958). Learning and instinct in animals. Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toda, K., & Platt, M. L. (2015). Animal cognition: Monkeys pass the mirror test. Current Biology, 25(2), R64–R66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomkins, D. M., & Sellers, E. M. (2001). Addiction and the brain: The role of neurotransmitters in the cause and treatment of drug dependence. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 164(6), 817–821.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tononi, G., Boly, M., Massimini, M., & Koch, C. (2016). Integrated information theory: From consciousness to its physical substrate. Nature Review of Neuroscience, 17(7), 450–461. doi:10.1038/nrn.2016.44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tononi, G., & Edelman, G. M. (1998). Consciousness and complexity. Science, 282(5395), 1846–1851.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tye, M. (1995). Ten problems of consciousness: A representational theory of the phenomenal mind (vol. 282). MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uzgiris, I. C., & Hunt, J. M. (1975). Assessment in infancy: Ordinal scales of psychological development. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Werf, Y. D., Witter, M. P., & Groenewegen, H. J. (2002). The intralaminar and midline nuclei of the thalamus. Anatomical and functional evidence for participation in processes of arousal and awareness. Brain Research Reviews, 39(2), 107–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Hoesen, G. W., Morecraft, R. J., & Vogt, B. A. (1993). Connections of the monkey cingulate cortex. In B. A. Vogt & M. Gabriel (Eds.), Neurobiology of cingulate cortex and limbic thalamus (pp. 249–284). Boston: Birkhäuser.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Vates, G. E., & Nottebohm, F. (1995). Feedback circuitry within a song-learning pathway. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 92(11), 5139–5143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V., Elshout, J. J., & Meijer, J. (1997). The generality vs domain-specificity of metacognitive skills in novice learning across domains. Learning and Instruction, 7(2), 187–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vicario, D. S. (1994). Motor mechanisms relevant to auditory-vocal interactions in songbirds. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 44(4–5), 265–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, B., Vogt, L., Nimchinsky, E., & Hof, P. (1997). Chapter VIII Primate cingulate cortex chemoarchitecture and its disruption in Alzheimer’s disease. Handbook of Chemical Neuroanatomy, 13, 455–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Von Economo, C., & Koskinas, G. (1925). The cytoarchitectonics of the adult human cortex. Vienna and Berlin: Julius Springer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waldmann, C., & Gu, O. (1993). The dopaminergic innervation of the pigeon caudolateral forebrain: Immunocytochemical evidence for a ‘prefrontal cortex’ in birds? Brain Research, 600(2), 225–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe, S. (2001). Discrimination of cartoons and photographs in pigeons: Effects of scrambling of elements. Behavioural Processes, 53(1–2), 3–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe, S., Sakamoto, J., & Wakita, M. (1995). Pigeons’ discrimination of paintings by Monet and Picasso. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 63(2), 165–174. doi:10.1901/jeab.1995.63-165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weir, A. A., Chappell, J., & Kacelnik, A. (2002). Shaping of hooks in New Caledonian crows. Science, 297(5583), 981.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wild, J., & Williams, M. (2000). Rostral wulst in passerine birds. I. Origin, course, and terminations of an avian pyramidal tract. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 416(4), 429–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xi, Z.-X., & Stein, E. A. (2002). GABAergic mechanisms of opiate reinforcement. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 37(5), 485–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, S., Bandler, R., & Davis, P. J. (1995). Brain stem integration of vocalization: Role of the nucleus retroambigualis. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74(6), 2500–2512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Soumya Iyengar .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Iyengar, S., Parishar, P., Mohapatra, A.N. (2017). Avian Cognition and Consciousness—From the Perspective of Neuroscience and Behaviour. In: Menon, S., Nagaraj, N., Binoy, V. (eds) Self, Culture and Consciousness. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-5777-9_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics