Skip to main content

The Neuroscience of Blame and Punishment

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Self, Culture and Consciousness

Abstract

In the last five years, a great deal has been learned about how human brains address the social problem of punishing wrongdoers. Although it is far too early to be confident that these insights will shed any practical light on criminal law or procedure, patterns are emerging that suggest a framework that someday could have significant legal and social consequences. In this chapter, we first survey the behavioural and theoretical evidence supporting the proposition that the willingness to blame then punish norm-violators is an evolved human trait. Then we sample the recent neuroscience literature on normative punishment, and follow that with a presentation of our neuropsychological model of blame and third-party punishment. We finish with a discussion of the potential implications a confirmed model might have for law and policy.

This chapter is adapted from our paper, Krueger, F. and Hoffman, M. (2016) The emerging neuroscience of third-party punishment, Trends in Neuroscience, 39(8): 499–501, and from Hoffman, M. 2014. The Punisher’s Brain: The Evolution of Judge and Jury (Cambridge University Press).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The most famous was Lorenz and Wilson  (2002), who wrote a whole book about it.

  2. 2.

    Many behavioural studies have confirmed that several primate species, and even some non-primate mammals, have a kind of empathy. To the extent that mirror neurons are involved with empathy, these behavioural results converge with experiments showing many of these same empathising social animals have mirror neurons. Indeed, some theorists have speculated that the essential imitative function of what we call mirror neurons may be a widespread neuronal phenomenon across many species.

  3. 3.

    Hume (1968) made this argument—that the roots of morality lie in our ability to socialise our own desires, i.e. have empathy—200 years ago.

  4. 4.

    A well-recognised statistical measure of the agreement between lists of rankings is called Kendall’s W (K w ), which measures not just differences in the rankings but also the degree of those differences. A K w of 1 means the lists match perfectly. A K w of 0 means no more matching than would expected of random ranking. A K w of 0.5 is typically described as ‘moderate agreement’. Subjects in the Robinson and Darley blameworthiness experiments averaged a K w ranging from 0.88 to 0.95. The only experimental behavioural tasks that even remotely approach this level of almost total agreement are things like asking visually unimpaired subjects to rate the brightness of objects (K w  = 0.95) and asking subjects to rate faces by how much pain they are feeling (K w  = 0.97).

  5. 5.

    Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) uses electromagnetic energy, focused into a small diameter, to temporarily disrupt the electrical impulses of neurons in regions of the brain fairly close to the surface.

  6. 6.

    A first model was published by Buckholtz and Marois (2012).

  7. 7.

    Note 5 supra.

  8. 8.

    Whether neuroscience will ever change our folk-psychological notions about things like personhood and responsibility—and, more radically, whether it might even disprove some of those notions—is the subject of considerable debate. Compare Greene and Cohen 2004 (‘[W]hen the mechanical nature of human decision-making is fully appreciated… the idea of distinguishing the truly, deeply guilty from those who are merely victims of neuronal circumstance will, we submit, seem pointless.’) with Morse (2015) (‘At present, neuroscience has little to contribute to more just and accurate criminal law policy, doctrine and individual case adjudication [and] no radical transformation of criminal justice is likely to occur….’). We discuss in the balance of this section the possibility that the neuroscience of punishment may someday lead to less dramatic, but no less important, changes in the law.

  9. 9.

    132 S. Ct. 2455 (2012).

  10. 10.

    Roper v. Simmons, 543 U.S. 551 (2008).

  11. 11.

    Montgomery v. Louisiana, 136 S. Ct. 718 (2016).

  12. 12.

    Hoffman, supra note 30, at 334–47.

  13. 13.

    Remember, though, that this data showing that different judges impose wildly different sentences on similar defendants in similar cases is not at all inconsistent with the remarkable uniformity with which humans blame. Note 5 supra.

  14. 14.

    In a follow-up study, researchers were able to improve results, but only marginally, by changing the definitions of these two problematic mental states (Ginther et al. 2014).

References

  • Anderson, J., Kling, J. R., & Stith, K. (1999). Measuring inter-judge sentencing disparity before and after the federal sentencing guidelines. Journal of Law and Economics, 42(S1), 271–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baird, J., & Astington, J. (2004). The role of mental state understanding in the development of moral cognition and moral action. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 103, 37–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barbey, A. K., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. (2009). An evolutionarily adaptive neural architecture for social reasoning. Trends in Neuroscience, 32(12), 603–610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bellucci, G., Chernyak, S., Hoffman, M., Deshpande, G., Dal Monte, O., Knutson, K. M., et al. (2017). Effective connectivity of brain regions underlying third-party punishment: Functional MRI and Granger causality evidence. Social Neuroscience, 12(2), 124–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, S. J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nature Review of Neuroscience, 9(4), 267–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boehm, C. (2012). Costs and benefits in hunter-gatherer punishment. Behavoiur Brain Science, 35(1), 19–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., Gintis, H., Bowles, S., & Richerson, P. J. (2003). The evolution of altruistic punishment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 100(6), 3531–3535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bressler, S. L., & Menon, V. (2010). Large-scale brain networks in cognition: Emerging methods and principles. Trends in Cognitive Science, 14(6), 277–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, D. (1991). Human Universals. McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buckholtz, J. W., Asplund, C. L., Dux, P. E., Zald, D. H., Gore, J. C., Jones, O. D., et al. (2008). The neural correlates of third-party punishment. Neuron, 60(5), 930–940.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckholtz, J. W., & Marois, R. (2012). The roots of modern justice: Cognitive and neural foundations of social norms and their enforcement. Nature Neuroscience, 15(5), 655–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckholtz, J. W., Martin, J. W., Treadway, M. T., Jan, K., Zald, D. H., Jones, O., et al. (2015). From blame to punishment: Disrupting prefrontal cortex activity reveals norm enforcement mechanisms. Neuron, 87(6), 1369–1380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (2012). Empathy in primates and other mammals. In J. Decety (Ed.), Empathy: From bench to bedside (pp. 87–106). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Weerd, H., & Verbrugge, R. (2011). Evolution of altruistic punishment in heterogeneous populations. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 290, 88–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(2), 63–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Gachter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415(6868), 137–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gachter, S., Renner, E., & Sefton, M. (2008). The long-run benefits of punishment. Science, 322(5907), 1510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ginther, M. R., Bonnie, R. J., Hoffman, M. B., Shen, F. X., Simons, K. W., Jones, O. D., et al. (2016). Parsing the behavioral and brain mechanisms of third-party punishment. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(36), 9420–9434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ginther, M. R., Shen, F. X., Bonnie, R. J., Hoffman, M. B., Jones, O. D., Marois, R., et al. (2014). The language of mens rea. Vanderbilt Law Review, 67, 1327–1372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glass, L., Moody, L., Grafman, J., & Krueger, F. (2016). Neural signatures of third-party punishment: Evidence from penetrating traumatic brain injury. Social Cognition and Affective Neuroscience, 11(2), 253–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godin, J., & Davis, S. (1995). Who dares, benefits: Predator approach behavior in the guppy (Poecelia retuclata). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 259, 193–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, R. (2006). A retaliatory role for algal projectiiles. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 26, 419–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J., & Cohen, J. (2004). For the law, neuroscience changes nothing and everything. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B: Biological Science, 359(1451), 1775–1785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gummerum, M., & Chu, M. T. (2014). Outcomes and intentions in children’s, adolescents’, and adults’ second- and third-party punishment behavior. Cognition, 133(1), 97–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, W. D. (1964a). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, W. D. (1964b). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 17–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., et al. (2006). Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312(5781), 1767–1770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hetzer, M., & Sornette, D. (2013). The co-evolution of fairness preferences and costly punishment. PLoS ONE, 8(3), e54308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, M. (2014). The punisher’s brain. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. (1968). Enquiry concerning the principles of morals (C.W. Hendel, ed.). New York: The Liberal Arts Press. (Original work published in 1751).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalbe, E., Schlegel, M., Sack, A. T., Nowak, D. A., Dafotakis, M., Bangard, C., et al. (2010). Dissociating cognitive from affective theory of mind: A TMS study. Cortex, 46, 769–780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karniol, R. (1978). Children’s use of intention cues in evaluating behavior. Psychology Bulletin, 85, 76–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenward, B., & Osth, T. (2012). Enactment of third-party punishment by 4-year-olds. Frontiers Psychology, 3, 373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenward, B., & Osth, T. (2015). Five-year-olds punish antisocial adults. Aggressive. Behaviour, 41(5), 413–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiehl, K. A. (2006). A cognitive neuroscience perspective on psychopathy: Evidence for paralimbic system dysfunction. Psychiatry Research, 142(2–3), 107–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krasnow, M. M., Delton, A. W., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2016). Looking under the hood of third-party punishment reveals design for personal benefit. Psychological Science, 27(3), 405–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, F., & Hoffman, M. (2016). The emerging neuroscience of third-party punishment. Trends in Neuroscience, 39(8), 499–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, F., Hoffman, M., Walter, H., & Grafman, J. (2014). An fMRI investigation of the effects of belief in free will on third-party punishment. Social Cognition and Affective Neuroscience, 9(8), 1143–1149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., & Decety, J. (2007). The neural substrate of human empathy: Effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(1), 42–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lancaster, J. (1986). Primate social behavior and ostracism. In R. Masters & M. Gruter (Eds.), Ostracism: A social and biological phenomenon. Ecology and Sociobiology (special issue), 7, 215–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leopold, A., Krueger, F., dal Monte, O., Pardini, M., Pulaski, S. J., Solomon, J., et al. (2012). Damage to the left ventromedial prefrontal cortex impacts affective theory of mind. Social Cognition and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 871–880.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K., & Wilson, M. K. (2002). Man meets dog. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. W., & Berbesque, J. C. (2008). More ‘altruistic’ punishment in larger societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 275(1634), 587–592.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, A. A., Finger, E. C., Mitchell, D. G., Reid, M. E., Sims, C., Kosson, D. S., et al. (2008). Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(6), 712–720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McAuliffe, K., Jordan, J. J., & Warneken, F. (2015). Costly third-party punishment in young children. Cognition, 134, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michl, P., Meindl, T., Meister, F., Born, C., Engel, R. R., Reiser, M., et al. (2014). Neurobiological underpinnings of shame and guilt: A pilot fMRI study. Social Cognition and Affective Neuroscience, 9(2), 150–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morse, S. (2015). Criminal law and common sense: The perils and promises of neuroscience. Marquette Law Review, 99, 39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nishida, T., Hosaka, K., Nakamura, M., & Hamai, M. (1995). A within-group gang attack on a young adult male chimpanzee: Ostracism of an ill-mannered member? Primates, 6(2), 207–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(04), 515–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raihani, N. J., Grutter, A. S., & Bshary, R. (2010). Punishers benefit from third-party punishment in fish. Science, 327(5962), 171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riedl, K., Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2012). No third-party punishment in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 109(37), 14824–14829.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, P., & Darley, J. (2007). Intuitions of Justice: Implications for criminal law and policy. Southern California Law Review, 81, 1–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roos, P., Gelfand, M., Nau, D., & Carr, R. (2014). High strength-of-ties and low mobility enable the evolution of third-party punishment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. B: Biological Sciences, 281(1776), 20132661.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sebastian, C. L., Fontaine, N. M., Bird, G., Blakemore, S. J., Brito, S. A., McCrory, E. J., et al. (2012). Neural processing associated with cognitive and affective theory of mind in adolescents and adults. Social Cognition and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 53–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shen, F., Hoffman, M., Jones, O., Greene, J., & Marois, R. (2011). Sorting guilty minds. New York University Law Review, 86, 1306–1360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamada, M., Camerer, C. F., Fujie, S., Kato, M., Matsuda, T., Takano, H., et al. (2012). Neural circuits in the brain that are activated when mitigating criminal sentences. Nature Communications, 3, 759.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, L., Camprodon, J. A., Hauser, M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Saxe, R. (2010). Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 107(15), 6753–6758.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Frank Krueger .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Hoffman, M.B., Krueger, F. (2017). The Neuroscience of Blame and Punishment. In: Menon, S., Nagaraj, N., Binoy, V. (eds) Self, Culture and Consciousness. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-5777-9_13

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics