Skip to main content

Spatial Grounding

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Nature, Metaphor, Culture

Part of the book series: Cultural Linguistics ((CL))

  • 374 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter deeply relies on the observations of Chap. 8 on mental space evolvement, elaborating on the notion of grounding. Exploring the various forms of spatial grounding in folksongs is based on the presumption that the different configurations of spatial grounding have their metaphorical correspondences and take part in the construal schemas partly presented in the previous chapter. The analysis is focused on two issues, spatial deixis and nominal grounding, and how they govern the process of construal in folksongs. Distal deixis, which is frequent in a stanza-initial position, provides a mental distance between the speaker and the nature scene, which usually displays a fictive or future/past event. Proximal deixis, on the other hand, represents actual reality and mental proximity, and maps immediate experience and positive emotional attitude. The analysis of nominal grounding considers three formal possibilities: nouns with definite articles, proper nouns, and nouns without an article. Based on the investigation of the data, definite nouns in folksongs do not necessarily refer to an object, which is easily accessible in the actual discourse universe but, instead, to one that is good for the community, i.e., easily identified by the schematic features of the object. In another group of folksongs, nominal constructions come up as initials in the stanzas either in a repetitive form or as a list of ungrounded phrases. In this construal schema the inherent schematic features of the concepts designated by nominal expressions establish a rather opaque image of the discourse world, which has a great semantic potential, whereas their follow-up semantic function cannot be anticipated at the beginning of the text. Based on statistical analyses of folksongs, it can be concluded that the commonly-held meaning of proper nouns is valid only in restrained circumstances: in terms of grounding, they only partly serve as salient reference points but, more importantly, they ‘localize’ the represented events in order to ground them in the cognition of the cultural group.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    In Hungarian the future tense is usually expressed using the present tense, see Chap. 8).

  2. 2.

    In the folksong below the peonies represent girls, who, by bending onto the road, are in their maturity and eager to have a relationship or get married.

    Két szál pünkösdrózsa

    Kihajlott az útra;

    El akar hervadni;

    Nincs, ki leszakítsa.

    Two peonies

    Have bent onto the road

    They are about to fade,

    There is nobody to pick them.

    Nem az ám a rózsa,

    Ki a kertben nyílik,

    Hanem az a rózsa,

    Ki egymást szereti!

    A peony is not the one

    Who grows in the garden.

    But a peony is the one,

    Who love each other.

    (Love song 45)

  3. 3.

    The term consituation is employed in the sense ‘situational context’.

References

  • Evans, V. (2004). The structure of time. Language, meaning and temporal cognition. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A., & Hargreaves, D. (1992). The privilege of primacy. In D. L. Payne (Ed.), Pragmatics of word order flexibility. Typological studies in language (Vol. 22, pp. 83–116). Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katona, I. (1998). Líra [Lyrics]. In V. Voigt (Ed.), A magyar folklór (pp. 356–399). Budapest: Osiris Kiadó.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kodály, Z. (1959). Előszó [Foreword]. In Kerényi, G. (Ed.), A Magyar Népzene Tára IV. Párosítók (pp. 1–19). Budapest: Akadémiai Kiadó.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kodály, Z., & Vargyas, L. (1952/1991). A magyar népzene. A példatárt szerkesztette Vargyas Lajos (12. kiadás) [The Hungarian folk music. The collection was edited by Lajos Vargyas]. Budapest: Editio Musica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kothencz, G. S. (2012). Kognitív jelentéstörténeti vizsgálatok a magyar nyelv határozóragjai körében [Cognitive historical-semantic studies on adverbial suffixes in Hungarian]. Doctoral dissertation. Budapest: Eötvös Lóránd University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kövecses, Z. (2000). Metaphor and emotion: Language, culture, and body in human feeling. New York, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kripke, S. A. (1980). Naming and necessity. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. W. (1991). Foundations of cognitive grammar. Volume II: Descriptive application. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. W. (2002). Deixis and subjectivity. In F. Brisard (Ed.), Grounding: The epistemic footing of deixis and reference (pp. 1–28). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. W. (2008). Cognitive grammar: A basic introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Péter, M. (1991). A nyelvi érzelemkifejezés ezsközei és módjai [The devices and methods of emotional expression in language]. Budapest: Tankönyvkiadó Vállalat.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27–48). Hillsdale, New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, C., & Bernárdez, E. (2015). Space, time and space-time: Metaphors, maps and fusions. In F. Sharifian (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of language and culture (pp. 309–324). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tátrai, S. (2008). Narratív távolság – lírai közvetlenség [Narrative distance—lyrical immediacy]. In S. Tátrai & Tolcsvai Nagy, G. (Eds.), Szöveg, szövegtípus, nyelvtan (pp. 49–55). Budapest: Tinta Könyvkiadó.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tátrai, S. (2010). Áttekintés a deixisről [Overview on deixis]. Magyar Nyelvőr, 134, 211–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolcsvai Nagy, G. (1996). A magyar nyelv stilisztikája [The stylistics of the Hungarian language]. Budapest: Nemzeti Tankönyvkiadó.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolcsvai Nagy, G. (2004). A nyelvi variancia kognitív leírása és a stílus (Egy kognitív stíluselmélet vázlata) [Style and a cognitive linguistic description of language diversity. Outline of a cognitive theory of style] In L. Büky (Ed.), A mai magyar nyelv leírásának újabb módszerei VI (pp. 143–160). Szeged: SZTE Általános Nyelvészeti Tanszék, Magyar Nyelvészeti Tanszék.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolcsvai Nagy, G. (2008). A tulajdonnév jelentése [The meaning of proper name]. In A. Bölcskei, & N. Császi Ildikó (Eds.), Név és valóság. A VI. Magyar Névtudományi Konferencia előadásai (Balatonszárszó, 2007. június 22–24, pp. 557–564). Budapest: KRE BTK Magyar Nyelvtudományi Tanszéke.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tribushinina, E. (2008). Cognitive reference points: Semantics beyond the prototypes in adjectives of space and colour. Doctoral Thesis. LOT, Utrecht: Leiden University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Langendonck, W. (2007). Theory and typology of proper names. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vermes, A. P. (2003). Proper names in translation: An explanatory attempt. Across Languages and Cultures, 4(1), 89–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vom Bruck, G., & Bodenhorn, B. (Eds.) (2006). The anthropology of names and naming. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Judit Baranyiné Kóczy .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Baranyiné Kóczy, J. (2018). Spatial Grounding. In: Nature, Metaphor, Culture. Cultural Linguistics. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-5753-3_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-5753-3_9

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Singapore

  • Print ISBN: 978-981-10-5752-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-981-10-5753-3

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics