Advertisement

Moral Psychology of Shame in Early Confucian Philosophy

  • Bongrae SeokEmail author
Chapter

Abstract

In Western traditions of virtue ethics, specifically in the Greco-Roman tradition of aretaic moral excellence, a virtue is characterized as a carefully developed and refined inner ability that reflects the personal and practical excellence of a moral agent. It is a fully developed disposition that comes out of careful process of cultivation and results in the well-rounded character and the flourishing life of a person.

Keywords

Moral Agent Moral Ideal Individualist Culture Moral Emotion Moral Significance 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgements

The research for this paper was supported by the grant provided by the Neag Professorship of Alvernia University. The author wishes to express his gratitude to the generous support. The author also wishes to thank an anonymous reviewer and a copy reader for their detailed and constructive comments.

References

  1. Aristotle (1985). Aristotle’s nicomachean ethics (T. Irwin, Trans.). Indianapolis, IN: Hackett Publishing. Google Scholar
  2. Aristotle (2011). Eudemian ethics (A. Kenny, Trans.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Bagozzi, R. P., Verbeke, W., & Gavino, J. C., Jr. (2003). Culture moderates the self-regulation of shame and its effects on performance: The case of salespersons in the Netherlands, and the Philippines. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 219–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barkan, E. (2000). The guilt of nations: restitution and negotiating historical injustices. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
  5. Babcock, M. K., & John Sabini, J. (1990). On differing embarrassment from shame. European Journal of Social Psychology, 20, 151–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Battistella, E. (1990). Markedness: The evaluative superstructure of language. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
  7. Bedford, O. A. (2004). The individual experience of guilt and shame in chinese culture. Culture & Psychology, 10–1, 29–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bedford, O. A., & Hwang, K.-K. (2003). Guilt and shame in chinese culture: A cross-cultural framework from the perspective of morality and identity. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 33, 127–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Benedict, R. (1946). The chrysanthemum and the sword: patterns of japanese culture. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  10. Breugelmans, S. M., & Poortinga, Ype H. (2006). Emotion without a word: Shame and guilt among raramuri indians and rural javanese. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(6), 1111–1122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cairns, D. L. (1993). Aidos: The psychology and ethics of honour and shame in ancient greek literature. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  12. Camras, L. A., & Serah, F. S. (2004). Development, culture, and alternative pathways to self-conscious emotions: A commentary on tracy and robins. Psychological Inquiry, 15(2), 166–170.Google Scholar
  13. Chiu, L.-H. (1986). Locus of control in intellectual situations in american and chinese school children. International Journal of Psychology, 21, 167–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Chu, C. L. (1972). On the shame orientation of the chinese from the interrelationship among society, individual, and culture. In Y. Li, K.-S. Yang (eds.), Symposium on the character of the chinese, an interdisciplinary approach, in Chinese, (pp. 85–125). Taipei: Institute of Ethnology, Academia Sinica.Google Scholar
  15. Crittenden, K. S. (1989). Attributional styles of asian and american students: A four-country study. St. Louis MI: Paper presented at the Midwest Sociological Society.Google Scholar
  16. Creighton, M. R. (1990). Revisiting shame and guilt cultures: A forty-year pilgrimage. Ethos, 18, 279–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Crystal, D. S., Gerrod Parrott, W., Okazaki, Y., & Watanabe, H. (2001). Examining relations between shame and personality among university students in the United States and Japan: A developmental perspective. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 113–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Confucius (1979). The analects. (D. C. Lau, Trans). New York, NY: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
  19. Dodds, E. R. (1951). The greeks and the irrational. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  20. Eberhard, W. (1967). Guilt and sin in traditional china. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  21. Frijda, N. H. & Mesquita, B. (1994). The Social Roles and Functions of Emotions. In S. Kitayama H. R. Markus (Eds.), Emotion and cultures: empirical studies of mutual influence (pp. 51–87). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  22. Fung, H. (1999). Becoming a moral child: The socialization of shaming among young chinese children. Ethos, 27, 180–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Fung, H., & Chen, E. C.-H. (2001). Across time and beyond Skin: Self and transgression in the everyday socialization of shame among taiwanese preschool children. Social Development, 10, 420–437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Geaney, J. (2004). Guarding moral boundaries: shame in early confucianism. Philosophy East & West, 54(2), 113–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gilbert, P. & Andrews, B. (1998). Preface. In P. Gilbert & B. (Eds.), Andrews in shame: Interpersonal behaviour, psychopathology, and culture (pp. v–vii). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Grimaldi, W. M. A. (Ed.). (1998). Aristotle rhetoric II: A commentary. New York, NY: Fordham University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Hamid, P. N. (1994). Self-Monitoring, locus of control, and social encounters of Chinese and New Zealand students. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 25, 353–368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York, NY: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Henning, A. (1989). Markedness-the first 150 years. In O. M. Tomić (Ed.), Markedness in synchrony and diachrony (pp. 11–46). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  30. Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  31. Hsieh, T. T., Shybut, J., & Lotsof, E. J. (1969). Internal versus external control and ethnic group membership. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 122–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hultberg, P. (1988). Shame: A hidden emotion. Journal of Analytical Psychology, 33, 109–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Huang, H.-C., Hwang, K.-K., & Ko, Y.-H. (1983). “Life stress, attribution style, social support and depression among university students” [in Chinese]. Acta Psychologica Taiwanica, 25, 31–47.Google Scholar
  34. Jakobson R. (1929). Remarques sur L’evolution Phonologique du Russe Comparée à celle des autres Langues Slaves. Travaux du Cercle Linguistique de Prague 2.Google Scholar
  35. Jakobson, R. (1968). Child Language, Aphasia and Phonological Universals (R. K Allan, Trans). The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
  36. Kendler, K. S., Hettema, J. M., Butera, F., Gardner, C. O., & Prescott, C. A. (2003). Life event dimensions of loss, humiliation, entrapment, and danger in the prediction of onsets of major depression and generalized anxiety. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 789–796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Keltner, D., & Harker, L. (1998). The forms and functions of the nonverbal signal of shame. In Pl Gilbert & B. Andrews (Eds.), Shame: Interpersonal behavior, psychopathology, and culture (pp. 78–98). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  38. Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Kurokawa, M. (2000). Culture, emotion and well-being: good feelings in Japan and the United States. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 93–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Matsumoto, H. (1995). Culture, self and emotion: A cultural perspective on ‘Self-Conscious’ emotions. In J. P. Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment and pride (pp. 439–464). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  40. Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and Self-Criticism in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1245–1267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kondo, D. K. (1990). Crafting selves: power, gender, and discourses of identity in a Japanese workplace. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  42. Konstan, D. (2003). Shame in ancient greece. Social Research, 70(4), 1031–1060.Google Scholar
  43. Konstan, D. (2006). The emotions of the ancient greeks: studies in Aristotle and Greek literature. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Lansky, M. R. (1996). Shame and suicide in sophocles’ ajax. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 65, 761–786.Google Scholar
  45. Lee, F., Hallahan, M., & Herzog, T. (1996). Explaining real life events: how culture and domain shape attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 732–741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. New York, NY: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  47. Luyten, P., Corveleyn, J., & Fontaine, J. R. J. (1998). The relationship between religiosity and mental health: distinguishing between shame and guilt. Mental Health, Religion, and Culture, 1–2, 165–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Mascolo, M. F., Fischer, K. W. & Li, J. (2003). Dynamic development of component systems of emotions: Pride, Shame, and Guilt in China and the United States. In R. J. Davidson, K. Scherer & H. H Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective science (pp. 375–408). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  49. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Mencius (1970). The mencius, (D. C. Lau, Trans.). New York, NY: Penguin Classics.Google Scholar
  51. Menon, U. & Schweder R.A. (1994) Kali’s Tongue: Cultural Psychology and the Power of Shame in Orissa, India. In S., Kitayama & H.R., Markus (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence (pp. 241–82). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  52. Morris, M., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 949–971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Morris, M., Nisbett, R., & Peng, K. (1995). Causal understanding across domains and cultures. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: a multidisciplinary debate (pp. 577–612). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  54. Nemesius (2009). Nemesius: On the nature of man (P. Van Der Eijk & R. W. Sharples, Trans.) Liverpool: Liverpool University Press.Google Scholar
  55. Nisbett, R. (2003). The geography of thought: how asians and westerners think differently and why. New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
  56. Ng, M. (1981). Internal shame as moral sanction. Journal of Chinese Philosophy, 8, 75–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: Theory and evidence. Psychological Review, 91, 347–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Richard, W. (1981). Moral behavior in chinese society: a theoretical perspective. In R. Wilson, S. Greenblatt, & A. A. Wilson (Eds.), Moral behavior in chinese society (pp. 1–20). New York, NY: Praeger.Google Scholar
  59. Riezler, K. (1943). Comment on the social psychology of shame. American Journal of Sociology, 48(4), 457–465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Roetz, H. (1993). Confucian ethics of the axial age. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
  61. Romney, A. K., & Moore, C. C. (1997). Cultural universals: Measuring the semantic structure of emotion terms in english and japanese. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 94, 5489–5494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Rozin, P. (2003). Five potential principles for understanding cultural difference in relation to individual differences. Journal of Research in Psychology, 37, 273–283.Google Scholar
  63. Sabini, J., & Silver, M. (1997). In defense of shame: shame in the context of guilt and embarrassment. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 27, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Santangelo, P. (1992). Human Conscience and Responsibility in Ming-Qing China. East Asian History, 4, 31–80.Google Scholar
  65. Scheff, T. (1997). Shame in social theory. In M. R. Lansky & A. P. Morrison (Eds.), The widening scope of shame (pp. 205–300). Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.Google Scholar
  66. Scheff, T. & Retzinger, S. M. (1991/2001). Emotions and violence: Shame and rage in destructive conflicts. Lincoln Nebraska: iUniverse.Google Scholar
  67. Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1061–1086.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Shun, K. (2001). Self and self-cultivation in early confucian thought. In B. Mou (Ed.), Two roads to wisdom? chinese and analytic philosophical traditions, Vol. 229 (44). Peru: Open Court.Google Scholar
  69. Simpson, J., & Weiner, E. (1989). The oxford english dictionary. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  70. Solomon, R., & Flores, F. (2003). Building trust, business, politics, and relationship, and life. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Stipek, D. (1998). Difference between Americans and Chinese in the circumstances evoking pride, shame, and guilt. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 79, 616–629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Swartz, M. J. (1988). Shame, culture, and status among the swahili of mombasa. Ethos, 16, 21–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Tangney, J. P. (1991). Moral affect: The good, the bad, and the ugly. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(4), 598–607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Tangney, J. P. (1993). Shame and guilt. In C. G. Costello (Ed.), Symptoms of depression (pp. 161–180). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  75. Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P. E. Hill-Barlow, D., Marshall, D. & Gramzow, R. (1996) Relation of shame and guilt to constructive versus destructive responses to anger across the lifespan. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology 70(4):797–809.Google Scholar
  76. Tangney, J., & Dearing, R. L. (2002). Shame and guilt. Emotions and social behavior. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  77. Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P. E., Flectcher, C. & Gramzov, R. (1992a). Shamed into anger? The relation of shame and guilt to anger and self-reported aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 62(4): 669–75.Google Scholar
  79. Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P. E., & Gramzov, R. (1992b). Proneness to shame, proneness to guilt, and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101(3), 469–478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Thrane, G. (1979). Shame and the construction of the self. The Annual of Psychoanalysis, 7, 321–341.Google Scholar
  81. Tomkins, S. S. (1963). Affect/Imagery/Consciousness, Vol. 2: The Negative Affects. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
  82. Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2006). Appraisal antecedents of shame and guilt: support for a theoretical model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1339–1351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Triandis, H. C. (1988). Collectivism versus individualism: a reconceptualization of a basic concept in cross-cultural psychology. In G. K. Verma & C. Bagley (Eds.), Cross-cultural studies of personality, attitudes, and cognition (pp. 60–95). London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  84. Triandis, H. C. (1993). Collectivism and individualism as cultural syndromes. Cross—cultural Research, 27, 155–180.Google Scholar
  85. Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
  86. Trubetzkoy, N. (1969). Principles of phonology. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  87. Tsai, J. L. (1996). Cultural and contextual influences on the emotional responding of chinese-american and european-american couples during conflict. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
  88. Tsai, J. L. (2006). Cultural differences in the valuation of shame and other complex emotions. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
  89. Tseng, W.-S. (1972). On chinese national character, from the viewpoint of personality development.” [in Chinese], In Y. Li & K.-S. Yang (Eds.), The character of chinese: An interdisciplinary approach [in Chinese] (pp. 227–350). Taipei: Institute of Ethnology, Academia Sinica.Google Scholar
  90. Tu, W.-M. (1979). Humanity and self-cultivation: Essays in confucian thought. Berkeley, CA: Asian Humanities Press.Google Scholar
  91. Van Norden, B. W. (2002). The emotion of shame and the virtue of righteousness in mencius. Dao, A Journal of Comparative Philosophy, 1, 45–77.Google Scholar
  92. Wagner, U., N’Diave, K., Ethofer, T., & Vuilleumier, P. (2011). Guilt-Specific processing in the prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 2461–2470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Wallbott, H. G., & Scherer, K. R. (1995). Cultural determinants in experiencing shame and guilt. In J. P. Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: the psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 465–487). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  94. Waugh, L. (1982). Marked and unmarked: A choice between unequals in semiotic structure. Semiotica, 38, 299–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Weber, M., Baehr, P, & Wells, G. (1905/2002). The Protestant ethic and the “Spirit” of capitalism. New York, NY: Penguin.Google Scholar
  96. Weiner, B. (1974). An achievement motivation and attribution theory. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.Google Scholar
  97. Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Weisfeld, G. E. (1999). “Darwinian Analysis of the Emotion of Pride/Shame.” In J. M. G. van der Dennen, D. Smillie & D. R. Wilson (Eds.), The darwinian heritage and sociobiology (pp. 319–33). Westport, Conn.:Praeger Publishers/Greenwood Publishing Group Inc.Google Scholar
  99. Williams, B. (1993). Shame and necessity. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  100. Wilkan, U. (1984). Shame and honour: A contestable pair. Man, 19, 635–652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Wilson, R. (1980) Conformity and deviance regarding moral rules in chinese society: a socialization perspective. In A. Kleinman, & T.-Y. Lin (Eds)., Normal and abnormal behavior in chinese culture. pp. 117–36, Dordrecht Holland: D. Reidel.Google Scholar
  102. Yang, K.-S., & Ho, D. Y. F. (1988). The Role of Yuan in chinese social life: A conceptual and empirical analysis. In A. C. Paranjpe, D. Y. F. Ho, & R. W. Rieber (Eds.), Asian Contributions to Psychology (pp.263–81). New York, NY: Praeger Publishers.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd. and Higher Education Press 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Humanities/PhilosophyAlvernia UniversityReadingUSA

Personalised recommendations