Skip to main content

Perceiving Affordances and Social Cognition

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Perspectives on Social Ontology and Social Cognition

Part of the book series: Studies in the Philosophy of Sociality ((SIPS,volume 4))

Abstract

Navigating successfully through the environment requires perceiving what action possibilities the ecological environment affords to us. Though ecological affordances have been discussed intensively in the Gibsonian tradition, little attention has been paid to the role that social cognition plays for our perception of ecological affordances. The present chapter aims to fill this gap in the debate. In a first step, I will provide a relational account of affordances according to which perceiving affordances are perceiving one’s own action possibilities (i.e. the ‘animal relatum’) in relation to particular aspects of the environment (i.e. the ‘environment relatum’), including physical, intentional, and institutional aspects. In a second step, I discuss the role of social cognition for the perception of ecological affordances in social and institutional contexts. Here, I distinguish between ‘social cognition in a narrow sense’ that is required to understand the attitudes and intentions of a particular person in a social context and ‘social cognition in a broad sense’ that is required to understand the shared intention of a social group in an institutional context.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Other approaches to affordances deny the notion of negative affordances. Michaels (2003, p. 137), for example, defends the view that affordances need to be action related. Basically, Michaels distinguishes between two categories of ‘affordances’; (1) action-related affordances such as stairs that afford climbing and (2) affordances in which actions are conspicuously absent such as a cliff or a snake that might afford danger or particular substances that afford nutrition. Since danger and nutrition are not actions in the sense of movements that are coordinated in order to achieve some goal, Michaels does not rank them among affordances.

  2. 2.

    Of course, the perceived ‘flexibility’ of the use of an object’s physical aspects ‘for whatever purpose’ is relative to the perceiving system. That is, although the secateurs may, in general, be perceived as being heavy and big enough to be a tool for breaking the window, I may not perceive this sensorimotor affordance because I do not think I am strong enough to pick up and throw the secateurs.

  3. 3.

    This does not necessarily presuppose the possession of a linguistic concept of pork, but only the capability to categorize pork according to specific physical aspects (colour, consistency etc.). Already 11-month-olds have been found to be able to categorize ecological objects (Pauen 2002).

  4. 4.

    That is, social cognition in a broad sense also comes into play when the interacting agents form a shared intention outside of an institutional setting. This, however, is not the focus of the present investigation.

References

  • Bhalla, M., and D.R. Profitt. 1999. Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 25: 1076–1096.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. 1995. Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, S.R., I.A. Apperly, J. Chappell, C. Guthrie, and N. Cutting. 2011. Making tools isn’t child’s play. Cognition 119: 301–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borg, E. 2007. If mirror neurons are the answer, what was the question? Journal of Consciousness Studies 14: 5–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bratman, M.E. 1993. Shared intention. Ethics 104(1): 97–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., N. Akhtar, and M. Tomasello. 1998. Fourteen-through 18 month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development 21(2): 315–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cesari, P., F. Formenti, and P. Olivato. 2003. A common perceptual parameter for stair climbing for children, young and old adults. Human Movement Science 22: 111–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A. 2003. An outline of a theory of affordances. Ecological Psychology 15(2): 181–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clifton, R.K., D.W. Muir, D.H. Ashmead, and M.G. Clarkson. 1993. Is visually guided reaching in early infancy a myth? Child Development 64: 1099–1110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G. 2010. Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind & Language 25: 141–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W.A., M.K. Johnson, C.L. Raye, J.C. Gatenby, J.C. Gore, and M.R. Banaji. 2004. Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces. Psychological Science 15: 806–813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A.H., and V.J. Steffen. 1984. Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46(4): 735–754.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elsner, B., and S. Pauen. 2007. Social learning of artifact function in 12- and 15-month-olds. European Journal of Developmental Psychology 4(1): 80–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiebich, A., and Gallagher, S. 2012. Joint attention in joint action. Philosophical Psychology, ifirst, 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Field, J. 1976. The adjustment of reaching behavior to object distance in early infancy. Child Development 47(1): 304–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fröse, T., and E.A. Di Paolo. 2011. The enactive approach. Theoretical sketches from cell to society. Pragmatics and Cognition 19(1): 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. 2005. How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. [1979] 1986. The theory of affordances. In Ecological approach to visual perception, pp. 127–143. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E.J., and R.D. Walk. 1960. The “visual cliff”. Scientific American 202: 67–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. 2006. Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of mindreading. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A. 2003. The theory theory as an alternative to the innateness hypothesis. In Chomsky and his critics, ed. L. Antony and N. Hornstein, 238–254. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. 1989. Affordances and the body: An intentional analysis of Gibson’s ecological approach to visual perception. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior 19: 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. 2001. Ecological psychology in context: James Gibson, Roger Barker, and the legacy of William James’ radical empiricism. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmarck, T.W., S.B. Manuck, and J.R. Jennings. 1990. Social support reduces cardiovascular reactivity to psychological challenge: A laboratory model. Psychosomatic Medicine 52: 42–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kono, T. 2009. Social affordances and the possibility of ecological linguistics. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science 43: 356–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, M.H., V.S.Y. Kwan, A. Cheung, and S.T. Fiske. 2005. Stereotype content model explains prejudice for an envied outgroup: Scale of anti-Asian American stereotypes. Personality and Social Bulletin 31: 34–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macrae, C.N., and G.V. Bodenhausen. 2000. Social cognition: Thinking categorically about others. Annual Review of Psychology 51: 93–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N. 1995. Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology 31: 838–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michaels, C.F. 2003. Affordances: Four points of debate. Ecological Psychology 15(2): 135–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moses, L.J., D.A. Baldwin, J.G. Rosicky, and G. Tidball. 2001. Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at twelve and eighteen months. Child Development 72: 718–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pauen, S. 2002. Evidence for knowledge-based categorization in infancy. Child Development 73: 116–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J. 1999. Metakognition und Introspektion in entwicklungspsychologischer Sicht: Studien zur “Theory of Mind” und “Simulation”. In Hundert Jahre Institut für Psychologie und Würzburger Schule der Denkpsychologie, ed. W. Janke and W. Schneider, 411–431. Göttingen: Hofgrede.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. [1970] 2003. Meine Theorie der geistigen Entwicklung. Weinheim: Beltz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proffitt, D.R., M. Bhalla, R. Gossweiler, and J. Midgett. 1995. Perceiving geographical slant. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 2: 409–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proffitt, D.R., J. Stefanucci, T. Banton, and W. Epstein. 2003. The role of effort in perceived distance. Psychological Science 1: 110–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H. 2008. Pretence as individual and collective intentionality. Mind & Language 23(5): 499–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H., F. Warneken, and M. Tomasello. 2008. The sources of normativity: Young children’s awareness of the normative structure of games. Developmental Psychology 44(3): 875–881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakoczy, H., F. Warneken, and M. Tomasello. 2009. Young children’s selective learning of rule games from reliable and unreliable models. Cognitive Development 24(1): 61–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, E.S. 1996. Encountering the world. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, R. 2005. Against simulation: The argument from error. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(4): 174–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schnall, S., K.D. Harber, J.K. Stefanucci, and D.R. Proffitt. 2008. Social support and the perception of geographical slant. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 44: 1246–1255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schnall, S., J.R. Zadra, and D.R. Proffitt. 2010. Direct evidence for the economy of action: Glucose and the perception of geographical slant. Perception 39: 464–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. 1990. Collective intentions and actions. In Intentions in communication, ed. P. Cohen, J. Morgan, and M. Pollack, 401–415. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. 2011. Making the social world. The structure of human civilization. Oxford: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen, T.A. 2003. Affordances as properties of the animal-environment system. Ecological Psychology 15(2): 115–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Striano, T., and P. Rochat. 2000. Emergence of selective social referencing in infancy. Infancy 1(2): 253–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. 1999a. The cultural ecology of young children’s interactions with objects and artifacts. In Ecological approaches to cognition. Essays in honor of Ulric Neisser. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. 1999b. The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Triesch, J., C. Teuscher, G. Deák, and E. Carlson. 2006. Gaze following: Why (not) learn it? Developmental Science 9(2): 125–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tuomela, R. 2010. The philosophy of sociality. Oxford: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warren, W.H. 1984. Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 10: 683–703.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williamson, R.A., V.K. Jaswal, and A.N. Meltzoff. 2010. Learning the rules: Observation and imitation of a sorting strategy by 36-month-old children. Developmental Psychology 46(1): 57–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I acknowledge the support provided by the Humboldt Foundation and Albert Newen for a research stay in Memphis, USA, where I finished this chapter. I would like to thank Shaun Gallagher, Steve Butterfill, Nivedita Gangopadhyay, John Michael, and one anonymous reviewer for helpful comments on an earlier draft of this chapter as well as Deborah Tollefsen and the auditory of the colloquium of the Philosophy Department of the University of Memphis for fruitful discussions. Finally, I would like to thank Adrian Smith and Joel Krueger for organizing a conference on affordances at the Center for Subjectivity Research in Copenhagen, 2010, which inspired me to dip deeper into that debate.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anika Fiebich .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Fiebich, A. (2014). Perceiving Affordances and Social Cognition. In: Gallotti, M., Michael, J. (eds) Perspectives on Social Ontology and Social Cognition. Studies in the Philosophy of Sociality, vol 4. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9147-2_11

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics