Skip to main content

Mountaineering or Ratcheting? Stone Age Hunting Weapons as Proxy for the Evolution of Human Technological, Behavioral and Cognitive Flexibility

Part of the Vertebrate Paleobiology and Paleoanthropology book series (VERT)

Abstract

Cultural, behavioral and cognitive evolution is often seen as cumulative and sometimes referred to in terms of a ratchet or the ratchet effect . In this contribution, I assess the value of the ratchet analogy as blanket explanation for the above aspects of human evolution . I use Stone Age weapon technologies as proxy for the evolution of human technological, behavioral and cognitive flexibility , and by doing so show that the ratchet analogy falls short of explaining human variability and complexity as reflected in the Stone Age archaeological record. Considering human cultural, behavioral and cognitive evolution from a theoretically constructed rugged landscape point of view, I suggest that mountaineering may be a more suitable analogy for the accumulation of human culture. In this scenario, culture and technology anchor societies within their respective evolutionary trajectories and fitness landscapes , and it more accurately reflects humans as ‘masters of flexibility’.

Keywords

  • Projectile technology
  • Bow-and-arrow
  • Spear
  • Spearthrower-and-dart
  • Cumulative culture
  • Cognition
  • Fitness landscapes

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • DOI: 10.1007/978-94-017-7426-0_12
  • Chapter length: 12 pages
  • Instant PDF download
  • Readable on all devices
  • Own it forever
  • Exclusive offer for individuals only
  • Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout
eBook
USD   99.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • ISBN: 978-94-017-7426-0
  • Instant PDF download
  • Readable on all devices
  • Own it forever
  • Exclusive offer for individuals only
  • Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout
Softcover Book
USD   129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
Hardcover Book
USD   129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
Fig. 12.1
Fig. 12.2
Fig. 12.3

References

  • Attenbrow, V., Robertson, G., & Hiscock, P. (2009). The changing abundance of backed artefacts in south-eastern Australia: A response to Holocene climate change? Journal of Archaeological Science, 36, 2765–2770.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Backwell, L., d’Errico, F., & Wadley, L. (2008). Middle Stone Age bone tools from the Howiesons Poort layers, Sibudu Cave, South Africa. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35, 566–1580.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barret, H. C. (2009). Where there is an adaptation, there is a domain: the form-function fit in information processing. In S. M. Platek & T. K. Schackelford (Eds.), Foundations in evolutionary cognitive neuroscience (pp. 97–116). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bradfield, J., & Lombard, M. (2011). A macrofracture study of bone points used in experimental hunting with reference to the South African Middle Stone Age. South African Archaeological Bulletin, 66, 67–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, A., Nevell, L., Yellen, J., & Hartman, G. (2006). Projectile technologies of the African MSA: Implications for modern human origins. In E. Hovers & S. Kuhn (Eds.), Transitions before the transition: Evolution and stability in the Middle Palaeolithic and Middle Stone Age (pp. 233–255). New York: Springer.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W. (1995). The thinking ape: Evolutionary origins of intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Carniero, R. L. (2003). Evolutionism in cultural anthropology: A critical history. Boulder: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattelain, P. (1997). Hunting during the Upper Paleolithic: Bow, spearthrower, or both? In H. Knecht (Ed.), Projectile Technology (pp. 213–240). New York: Plenum Press.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Colunga, E., & Smith, L. B. (2007). Flexibility and variability: Essential to human cognition and the study of human cognition. New Ideas in Psychology, 26, 170–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico, F., & Stringer, C. B. (2011). Evolution, revolution or saltation scenario for the emergence of modern cultures? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 1060–1069.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, J. (2005). Guns, germs and steel: A short history of everybody for the last 13,000 years. London: Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (2005). The origin of mind: Evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence. Washington: American Psychological Association.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Haidle, M. N. (2009). How to think a simple spear. In S. A. de Beaune, F. L. Coolidge, & T. Wynn (Eds.), Cognitive archaeology and human evolution (pp. 57–73). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidle, M. N. (2010). Working-memory capacity and the evolution of modern cognitive potential: Implications from animal and early human tool use. Current Anthropology, 51, S149–S166.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Haidle, M. N. (2012). How to Think Tools? A Comparison of Cognitive Aspects in Tool Behavior of Animals and During Human Evolution. Cognitive perspectives in tool behavior, Vol. 1. urn:nbn:de:bsz:21-opus-60146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2004). Archaeology, demography and cultural evolution: How adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses – the Tasmanian case. American Antiquity, 69, 197–214.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, C. S. (2005). Stratigraphic integrity of the Middle Stone Age levels at Blombos Cave. In F. d’Errico & L. Backwell (Eds.), From tools to symbols: From early hominids to modern humans (pp. 441–458). Johannesburg: Witwatersrand University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, C. S., & Dubreuil, B. (2011). The still bay and Howiesons Poort, 77–59 ka: Symbolic material culture and the evolution of the mind during the African Middle Stone Age. Current Anthropology, 52, 361–400.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Hovers, E., & Belfer-Cohen, A. (2006). Now you see it, now you don’t—modern human behaviour in the Middle Paleolithic. In E. Hovers & S. L. Kuhn (Eds.), Transitions before the transition: Evolution and stability in the Middle Paleolithic and Middle Stone Age (pp. 295–304). New York: Springer.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, S. (2006). Trajectories of change in the Middle Paleolithic in Italy. In E. Hovers & S. L. Kuhn (Eds.), Transitions before the transition: Evolution and stability in the Middle Paleolithic and Middle Stone Age (pp. 109–120). New York: Springer.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Lombard, M. (2005). Evidence of hunting and hafting during the Middle Stone Age at Sibudu Cave, KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa: A multianalytical approach. Journal of Human Evolution, 48, 279–300.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Lombard, M. (2006). First impressions of the functions and hafting technology of Still Bay pointed artefacts from Sibudu Cave. Southern African Humanities, 18(1), 27–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lombard, M. (2007). Evidence for change in Middle Stone Age hunting behaviour at Blombos Cave: Results of a macrofracture analysis. South African Archaeological Bulletin, 62, 62–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lombard, M. (2011). Quartz-tipped arrows older than 60 ka: Further use-trace evidence from Sibudu, KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Journal of Archaeological Science, 38, 1918–1930.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Lombard, M., & Haidle, M. N. (2012). Thinking a bow-and-arrow: Cognitive implications of Middle Stone Age bow and stone-tipped arrow technology. Cambridge Journal of Archaeology, 22, 237–264.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Lombard, M., & Pargeter, J. (2008). Hunting with Howiesons Poort segments: Pilot experimental study and the functional interpretation of archaeological tools. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35, 2523–2531.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Lombard, M., & Parsons, I. (2010). Fact or fiction? Behavioural and technological reversal after 60 ka in Southern Africa. South African Archaeological Bulletin, 65, 224–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lombard, M., & Parsons, I. (2011). What happened to the human mind after the Howiesons Poort? Antiquity, 85, 1433–1443.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Lombard, M., & Phillipson, L. (2010). Indications of bow and stone-tipped arrow use 64,000 years ago in KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Antiquity, 84, 635–648.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • McBrearty, S., & Tryon, C. (2005). From Acheulean to Middle Stone Age in the Kapthurin Formation, Kenya. In E. Hovers & S. L. Kuhn (Eds.), Transitions before the transition: Evolution and stability in the Middle Paleolithic and Middle Stone Age (pp. 257–277). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCall, G. S. (2007). Behavioral ecological models of lithic technological change during the later Middle Stone Age of South Africa. Journal of Archaeological Science, 34, 1738–1751.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Mellars, P. (2007). Rethinking the revolution: Eurasian and African perspectives. In P. Mellars, K. Boyle, O. Bar-Yosef, & C. Stringer (Eds.), Rethinking the revolution: New behavioural and biological perspectives on the origin and dispersal of modern humans (pp. 1–14). Cambridge: McDonald Institute Monographs.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, N., & Valladas, H. (2003). Reassessment of TL age estimates of burnt flint from the Paleolithic site of Tabun Cave, Israel. Journal of Human Evolution, 45, 401–409.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A. (2008). Foresight in cultural evolution. Biological Philosophy, 28, 243–255.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Mohapi, M. (2007). Rose Cottage Cave lithic points: Does technological change imply change in hunting techniques? South African Archaeological Bulletin, 62, 9–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parsons, I., & Lombard, M. (2011). The multi-disciplinary landscape of mid-late Upper Pleistocene archaeology. South African Archaeological Bulletin, 66, 178–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, A., Shennan, S., & Thomas, M. G. (2009). Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science, 324, 1298–1301.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Preutz, J. D., & Bertolani, P. (2007). Savanna chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, hunt with tools. Current Biology, 17, 412–417.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Riede, F. (2008). The Laacher See-eruption (12,920 BP) and material culture change at the end of the Allerød in Northern Europe. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35, 591–599.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Riegler, A. (2001). The cognitive ratchet: The ratchet effect as a fundamental principle in evolution and cognition. Cybernetics and Systems, 32, 411–427.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Rots, V., & Van Peer, P. (2006). Early evidence of complexity in lithic economy: Core-axe production, hafting and use at Late Middle Pleistocene site 8-B-11, Sai Island (Sudan). Journal of Archaeological Science, 33, 360–371.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Shea, J. J. (2006). The origins of lithic projectile point technology: Evidence from Africa, the Levant, and Europe. Journal of Archaeological Science, 33, 823–846.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Shea, J. J. (2009). The impact of projectile weaponry on Late Pleistocene hominin evolution. In J.-J. Hublin & M. P. Richards (Eds.), The evolution of Hominin Diets: Integrating approaches to the study of Palaeolithic subsistence (pp. 189–199). Leipzig: Springer Science.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Shea, J. J. (2011). Homo sapiens is as Homo sapiens was: Behavioural variability versus “behavioural modernity” in Paleolithic archaeology. Current Anthropology, 52, 1–35.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Shea, J. J., & Sisk, M. L. (2010). Complex projectile technology and Homo sapiens dispersal into western Eurasia. PaleoAnthropology, 2010, 100–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. (2001). Demography and cultural innovation: A model and its implications for the emergence of modern human culture. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 11, 5–16.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, H. (1896). The principles of sociology. New York: Appleton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thieme, H. (1997). Lower Paleolithic hunting spears from Germany. Nature, 385, 807–810.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Thieme, H. (1999). Altpaläolithische Holzgeräte aus Schöningen. Lkr. Helmstedt. Germania, 77, 451–487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 2405–2415.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. P., & Burkart, J. M. (2011). Social learning and evolution: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 1008–1016.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Villa, P., & Soriano, S. (2010). Hunting weapons of Neanderthals and early modern humans in South Africa. Journal of Anthropological Research, 66, 5–38.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Villa, P., Soriano, S., Teyssandier, N., & Wurz, S. (2010). The Howiesons Poort and MSA III at Klasies River Mouth, Cave 1A. Journal of Archaeological Science, 37, 630–655.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Wadley, L. (2005). Putting ochre to the test: Replication studies of adhesives that may have been used for hafting tools in the Middle Stone Age. Journal of Human Evolution, 49, 587–601.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Wadley, L. (2010). Were snares used in the Middle Stone age and does it matter? A review and case study from Sibudu, South Africa. Journal of Human Evolution, 58, 179–192.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Wadley, L., & Mohapi, M. (2008). A segment is not a monolith: Evidence from the Howiesons Poort of Sibudu, South Africa. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35, 2594–2605.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Wadley, L., Hodgskiss, T., & Grant, M. (2009). Implications for complex cognition from the hafting of tools with compound adhesives in the Middle Stone Age, South Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 106, 9590–9594.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wadley, L., Sievers, C., Bamford, M., Goldberg, P., Berna, F., & Miller, C. (2011). Middle Stone Age bedding construction and settlement patterns at Sibudu, South Africa. Science, 334, 1388–1391.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins, J., Schoville, B. J., Brown, K. S., & Chazan, M. (2012). Evidence for early hafted hunting technology. Science, 338, 942–946.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, V. M. E., Burke, A., & Lombard, M. (2014). Throwing spears and shooting arrows: Preliminary results of a pilot neuroarchaeological study. South African Archaeological Bulletin, 69, 199–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, S. (1932). The roles of mutation, inbreeding, crossbreeding and selection in evolution. Proceedings of the Sixth International Congress on Genetics, 1, 356–366.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I thank the ROCEEH group for the invitation and funding to participate in this discussion, and colleagues and friends who took the time to comment on the draft manuscript. My research is funded by the African Origins Platform of the National Research Foundation of South Africa. Opinions and mistakes remain my own.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marlize Lombard .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Lombard, M. (2016). Mountaineering or Ratcheting? Stone Age Hunting Weapons as Proxy for the Evolution of Human Technological, Behavioral and Cognitive Flexibility. In: Haidle, M., Conard, N., Bolus, M. (eds) The Nature of Culture. Vertebrate Paleobiology and Paleoanthropology. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-7426-0_12

Download citation