Skip to main content

Connectionist Models of Children’s Reading

  • Chapter
Handbook of Children’s Literacy

Abstract

This chapter focuses on connectionist models of reading development, and outlines several different ways in which connectionist modelling both has been, and could be, used to advance the theoretical understanding of reading development.

Several different developmental possibilities are distinguished in the context of a discussion of connectionist models. In connectionist terms literacy development could in principle be constrained by various distinct factors, including at least (a) the small or unrepresentative nature of the fragment of the spelling-to-sound mapping system early learners are exposed to; (b) the length of time a learning mechanism requires to develop a good model of the statistical structure of the language; (c) the slow development of the representations that allow the relevant generalisations to be learned, and (d) more general limitations in the representational capacity of the learning system throughout development. Computational modelling allows a clear and explicit distinction between, and exploration of, each of these factors.

In reality, of course, it is likely that limitations on literacy development are a composite of these and other factors, and that the factors interact with one another to define a rather complex developmental trajectory. But this is precisely the point at which connectionist modelling may make a useful contribution, because the examination of artificial learning systems may allow us to tease apart the contributions of the different elements in a way that will never be possible by empirical investigation alone.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 269.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 349.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 499.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1990). The adaptive character of thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R., & Milson, R. (1989). Human memory: An adaptive perspective. Psychological Review, 96, 703–719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R., & Schooler, L. J. (1991). Reflections of the environment in memory. Psychological Science. 2. 396–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ans, B., Carbonnel, S., & Valdois, S. (1998). A connectionist multiple-trace memory model for polysyllabic word reading. Psychological Review, 105, 678–723.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bechtel, W. & Abrahamsen, A. A. (1991). Connectionism and the Mind An Introduction to Parallel Processing in Networks. Cambridge MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D, Brooksher, R., Lemos, Z., Ogier, S., Zook, D., & Mostafapour, E. (2000). A connectionist approach to making the predictability of English orthography explicit to at-risk beginning readers. Evidence for alternative, effective strategies. Developmental Neuropsychology, 17, 241–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Besner, D., Twilley, L., McCann, R. S. & Seergobin, K. (1990). On the association between connectionism and data: Are a few words necessary? Psychological Review. 97, 432–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorising sounds and learning to read -causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. D. A. (1995). A rational analysis of reading: Spelling-to-sound translation is optimal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. D. A. (1987) Resolving inconsistency: A computational model of word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 23, 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. D. A. (1997). Connectionism, phonology, reading, and regularity in developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 59, 207–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. D. A. (1998). The endpoint of reading instruction The ROAR model. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy. Mahwah, NJ: LEA (pp. 121–138 )

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullmaria, J. A. (1995). Neural network models of reading Solving the alignment problem without Wickelfeatures. In J. P. Levy, D. Bairaktaris, J. A. Bullinaria, & D. Cairns (Eds.), Connectionist models of memory and language. London: UCL Press. (pp. 161–178 ).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullinaria, J. A. (1997). Modeling reading, spelling, and past tense learning with artificial neural networks. Brain and Language. 59. 236–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chater, N. (1995). Neural networks: the new statistical models of mind. In J. P. Levy, D. Bairaktaris, J. A. Bullinaria, & D. Cairns (Eds.), Connectionist models of memory and language. London: UCL Press. (pp. 207–227 ).

    Google Scholar 

  • Chater, N., & Oaksford, M. (1990). Autonomy, implementation and cognitive architecture: A reply to Fodor and Pylyshyn. Cognition, 34, 93–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chater, N., & Oaksford, M. (1999). Ten years of the rational analysis of cognition. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3. 56–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Curtis, Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and paralleldistributed-processing accounts. Psychological Review, 100, 589–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cortese, M. J. (1998). Revisiting serial position effects in reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 652–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J. L. (1993). Learning and development in neural networks: The importance of starting small. Cognition, 48, 71–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J., & Pylyshyn, Z. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis. Cognition, 28, 3–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • French, R. M., & Chater, N. (2002). Using noise to compute error surfaces in connectionist networks: A novel means of reducing catastrophic forgetting. Neural Computation, 14, 1755–1769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. E. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harm, M. W. (1998). Division of labor in a computational model of visual word recognition. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Southern California, August 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review, 106, 491–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C., Quinlan, P., Bolt, G., & Snolwing, M. J. (1995). Building phonological knowledge into a connectionist model of the development of word naming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10, 387–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Quinlan, P. (1991). Connectionism and learning to read: Steps towards a psychologically plausible model. Reading and Writing, 3, 59–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jared, D., McRae, K. & Seidenberg, M. S. (1990). The basis of consistency effects in word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 687–715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, D. S. (1991). Introduction to neural and cognitive modeling. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F., Seidenberg, M., Doi, L., McBride-Chang, C., & Peterson, A. (1996). On the basis of two subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 58, 157–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. F. (1995). The acquisition of the English past tense in children and multilayered connectionist networks. Cognition., 56, 271–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L. (1998). Connectionist models and Bayesian inference. In M. Oaksford & N. Chater (Eds.), Rational models of cognition. Oxford: OUP. (pp. 21–53 ).

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., & O’Reilly, R. C. (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102, 419–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLeod, P., Plunkett, K., & Rolls, E. T. (1998). Introduction to connectionist modelling of cognitive processes. Oxford: OUP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metsala, J. L., & Brown, G. D.A. (1998). Normal and dyslexic reading development: The role of formal models. In R. M. Joshi & C. Hulme (Eds.), Reading and Spelling: Development and disorders. Mahwah, NJ: LEA. (pp. 235–262 ).

    Google Scholar 

  • Metsala, J. L., Stanovich, K. E., & Brown, G. D.A. (1998). Regularity effects and the phonological deficit model of reading disabilities: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 279–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mozer, M. C., & Smolensky, P. (1989). Using relevance to reduce network size automatically. Connection Science, 1, 3–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norris, D. (1994). A quantitative model of reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 20, 1212–1232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (1998). (Eds.) Rational models of cognition. Oxford: OUP

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, C, & Ziegler, J. C. (2002). Cross-language computational investigation of the length effect in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 28, 990–1001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, W. A., Hay, I. M., & Smith, L. S. (1993). Lexicality and pronunciation in a simulated neural net. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 46, 193–205

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. & Prince, A. (1988). On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition, 28, 73–193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S. & Patterson, K. E. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103, 56–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plunkett, K., & Marchman, V. (1991). U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multi-layered perception: Implications for child language acquisition. Cognition, 38, 43–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plunkett, K., & Marchman, V. (1993). From rote learning to system building. Acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets. Cognition. 48. 21–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinlan, P. (1991). Connectionism and Psychology. London: Harvester.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rastle, K., & Coltheart, M. (1998). Whammies and double whammies: The effect of length on nonword reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 277–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rastle, K., & Coltheart, M. (1999). Serial and strategic effects in reading aloud. Journal of Experunental Psychology–Human Perception and Performance, 25, 482–503

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rastle, K., & Coltheart, M. (2000). Serial processing in reading aloud: Reply to Zorzi (2000). Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1232 - l235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E. & Williams, R. J. (1986). Learning internal presentations by error propagation. In D. E. Rumelhart and J. L. McClelland (Eds.), Parallel Distributed Processing Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (Vol.1). Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/ MiT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On Learning the Past Tenses of English Verbs. In I. L. McClelland & D. E. Rumelhart (Eds.), Parallel Distributed Processing Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (Vol. 2). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S. & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sejnowski, T. J. & Rosenberg, C. R. (1987). Parallel Networks that learn to pronounce English Text. Complex Systems, 1, 145–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquition. Cognition, 55, 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N. (1987). Towards a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science, 237, 1317–1323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.B., & Siegel, L S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, M., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (in press). Are developmental disorders like cases of adult brain damage? Implications from connectionist modelling. Behavioural and Brain Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Mullennix, J., Bijeljac-Babic, R., & Richmond-Welty, E. D. (1995). The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 124, 107–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G. C., Pennington, B. F., & Stone, G O. (1990). Word identification in reading and the promise of subsymbolic psycholinguistics. Psychological Review, 97, 488–522

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G. C., Pennington, B. F., & Stone, G. O. (2001). What do double dissociations prove? Cognitive Science, 25, 111–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weekes, B. S. (1997). Differential effects of number of letters on word and nonword naming latency.Quarterly Journal Of Experimental Psychology, 50, 439–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zorzi, M. (2000). Serial processing in reading aloud: No challenge for a parallel model. Journal of Experimental Psychology’ Human Perception and Performance, 26, 847–856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zorzi, M., Houghton, G., & Butterworth, B. (1998a). The development of spelling-sound relationships in a model of phonological reading. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13, 337–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zorzi, M., Houghton, G., & Butterworth, B. (1998b). Two routes or one in reading aloud? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1131–1161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Suggested Further Readings

  • Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review, 106, 491–528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, M., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (in press). Are developmental disorders like cases of adult brain damage? Implications from connectionist modelling. Behavioural and Brain Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Terezinha Nunes Peter Bryant

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2004 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Brown, G.D.A., Chater, N. (2004). Connectionist Models of Children’s Reading. In: Nunes, T., Bryant, P. (eds) Handbook of Children’s Literacy. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-1731-1_5

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-1731-1_5

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-90-481-6422-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-017-1731-1

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics