Skip to main content

The Prevalence of Salient-Features Coding in Choice-Reaction Tasks

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: NATO ASI Series ((ASID,volume 62))

Abstract

Stimulus-response translation plays a prominent role in the performance of choice-reaction tasks. Our previous work with spatial-precuing and symbolic-cuing tasks has provided evidence that the codes used for translation are based on the salient features of the stimulus and response sets. Responding is fastest for situations in which the salient features for the respective sets correspond. In this chapter, phenomena previously attributed to motor-programming processes or to direct perceptual-motor interactions are shown to be due to translational coding. These coding effects are important cognitive factors in human performance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   259.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

6. References

  • Bauer, D. W. , & Miller, J. (1982). Stimulus-response compatibility and the motor system.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,34A, 367–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cauraugh, J. H., & Horrell, J. F. (1989). Advance preparation of discrete motor responses: Nonmotoric evidence. Acta Psychologica, 72, 117–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G. , & Clark, H. H. (1971). Semantics in the perception of verticality.British Journal of Psychology. 63. 311–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dornier, L. A., & Reeve, T. G. (1990). Evaluation of commpatibility effects in the precuing of arm and direction parameters. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 61, 37–49.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, D. , & Kelso, J. A. S. (1980). Are movements prepared in parts? Not under compatible (nautralized) conditions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 475–495.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, P. C. (1990). Perceptual-motor processing in speech. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 343–362). Amsterdam: North-HoHand.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, P. C. & Meyer, D. E. (1984). Perceptual-motor processing of phonetic features in speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 153–178.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heister, G., Schroeder-Heister, P., & Ehrenstein, W. H. (1990). Spatial coding and spatio-anatomical mapping: Evidence for a hierarchical model of spatial stimulus-response compatibility. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 117–143). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klapp, S. T. (1977). Reaction time analysis of programmed control. Exercise and Sport Sciences Review. 5, 231–253.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Klapp, S. T. (1981). Motor programming is not the only process which can influence RT: Some thoughts on the Marteniuk and MacKenzie analysis. Journal of Motor Behavior, 13, 320–328.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Larish, D. D. (1986). Influence of stimulus-response translations on response programming: Examining the relationship of arm, direction, and extent of movement. Acta Psychologica, 61, 53–70.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Larish, D. D., & Frekany, G. A. (1985). Planning and preparing expected and unexpected movements: Reexamining the relationships of arm, direction, and extent of movement. Journal of Motor Behavior. 17, 168–189.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marteniuk, R. G. , & MacKenzie, C. L. (1981). Methods in the study of motor programming: Is it just a matter of simple vs. choice reaction time? A comment on Klapp et al. (1979). Journal of Motor Behavior. 13, 313–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meijer, 0. G. , & Roth, K. (1988). Complex movement behaviour: ‘The’ motor-action controversy. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michaels, C. F. (1988). S-R compatibility between response position and destination of apparent motion: Evidence of the detection of affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 231–240.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. (1982). Discrete versus continuous stage models of human information processing: In search of partial output. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 8, 273–296.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nicoletti, R., & Umilta, C. (1984). Right-left prevalence in spatial compatibility. Perception & Psychophvsics, 35, 333–343.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, G. M., & Laxar, K. (1973). Asymmetries in processing the terms‘right’and ‘left’.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100, 284–290.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W. & Reeve, T. G. (1985). Compatibility effects in the assignment of symbolic stimuli to discrete finger resonses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 623–639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W. , & Reeve, T. G. (1986). Salient-feature coding operations in spatial precuing tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 277–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W. , & Reeve, T. G. (1988). The acquisition of task-specific productions and modification of declarative representations in spatial-precuing tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 182–196.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W. , Reeve, T. G. , Weeks, D. J., Dornier, L. A. , & Van Zandt, T. (in press). Acquisition, retention, and transfer of response-selection skill in choice-reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning. Memory, and Cognition.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W., Van Zandt, T., Lu, C.-H., & Weeks, D. J. (1990, November). Stimulus-response compatibility for destination of apparent motion: Catching affordances or directional coding? Paper presented at the 31st annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans, LA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W. , Weeks, D. J., & Kelly, P. (1990). Performance with consonant-vowel and spatial-location stimulus and response sets: A salient-features account. Manuscript in preparation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reeve, T. G. , & Proctor, R. W. (1984). On the advance preparation of discrete finger responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 541–553.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reeve, T. G., & Proctor, R. W. (1985). Nonmotoric translation processes in the preparation of discrete finger responses: A rebuttal of Miller’s (1985) analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 234–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reeve, T. G. , & Proctor, R. W. (1990). The salient-features coding princple for spatial and symbolic-comaptibility effects. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 163–180). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A. (1980). Human movement initiation: Specification of arm, direction, and extent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 444–474.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A. (1983). The movement precuing technique: Assumptions, applications, and extensions. In R. A. Magill (Ed.), Memory and control of action (pp. 231–274). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Teichner, W. H., & Krebs, M. J. (1974). Laws of visual choice reaction time. Psychological Review. 81. 75–98.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Umiltà, C., Nicoletti, R. (1990). Spatial stimulus-response compatibility. In R. W Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 89–116). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weeks, D. J., & Proctor, R. W. (in press). Salient-features coding in the translation between orthogonal stimulus and response dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1991 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Proctor, R.W., Reeve, T.G. (1991). The Prevalence of Salient-Features Coding in Choice-Reaction Tasks. In: Requin, J., Stelmach, G.E. (eds) Tutorials in Motor Neuroscience. NATO ASI Series, vol 62. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-3626-6_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-3626-6_2

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-94-010-5609-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-011-3626-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics