Skip to main content

CAT: A Program of Comprehensive Abilities Testing

  • Chapter
Changing Assessments

Part of the book series: Evaluation in Education and Human Services ((EEHS,volume 30))

Abstract

If you decided to invest in the stock market, your first goal might be to look at various indicators of stock market performance. You would be unlikely to make a large investment based on a single indicator, such as the company’s profits or debit position, because a company’s performance depends on many variables. People are the same way. Their future performance depends on a number of variables, and any one indicator would be incomplete and probably misleading. Yet we make predictions about people’s performance on the basis of small numbers of indicators, sometimes a single one. This single indicator usually goes under the name of “an intelligence test,” “a test of mental abilities,” or “a scholastic aptitude test.”

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Alexander, P. 1985. Gifted and nongifted students’ perceptions of intelligence. Gifted Child Quarterly 29:137–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anthony, P. 1973. Race against time. New York: Tom Doherty Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B. 1986. Notes on the concept of intelligence. In What is intelligence? Contemporary viewpoints on its nature and definition, eds. R. J. Sternberg and D. K. Detterman, 23–27. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry, J. W. 1972. Radical cultural relativism and the concept of intelligence. In Mental tests and cultural adaptation, ed. L. J. Cronbach and P. Drenth. The Hague, Holland: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry, J. W. 1974. Radical cultural relativism and the concept of intelligence. In Culture and cognition: Readings in cross-cultural psychology, ed. J. W. Berry and P. R. Dasen, 225–29. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry, J. W. 1980. Cultural universality of any theory of human intelligence remains an open question. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:584–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binet, A., and T. Simon. 1905. Methodes nouvelles pour le diagnostic du niveau intellectuel des anormaux. L’Annee Psychologique 11:191–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binet, A., and T. Simon. 1916. The development of intelligence in children. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binet, A., and T. Simon. 1973. Classics in psychology: The development of intelligence in children. New York: Arno Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blum, M. L., and J. C. Naylor, eds. 1968. Industrial psychology: Its theoretical and social foundations. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. 1977. Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist 7:513–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L. 1978. Knowing when, where, and how to remember: A problem of metacognition. In vol. 1, Advances in instructional psychology, ed. R. Glaser. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L., J. D. Bransford, R. A. Ferrara, and J. C. Campione. 1983. Learning, remembering, and understanding. In Handbook of child psychology. 4th ed., ed. P. H. Mussen. Vol. 3, Cognitive development, ed. J. H. Flavell and E. M. Markman, 77–166. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterfield, E. C., C. Wambold, and J. M. Belmont. 1973. On the theory and practice of improving short-term memory. American Journal of Mental Deficiency 77:654–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campione, J. C., and A. L. Brown. 1979. Towards a theory of intelligence: Contributions from research with retarded children. In Human intelligence: Perspectives on its theory and measurement, ed. R. J. Sternberg and D. K. Detterman, 139–64. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cantor, N., and W. Mischel. 1979. Prototypes in person perception. In Advances in experimental social psychology, ed. L. Berkowitz. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. 1976. Psychometric tests as cognitive tasks: A new “structure of intellect.” In The nature of intelligence, ed. L. B. Resnick. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. 1980. Individual difference relations in psychometric and experimental cognitive tasks. Final Report NR 150-406 ONR. Chapel Hill, NC: L. L. Thurstone Psychometric Laboratory, University of North Carolina.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B. 1971. Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G., and H. A. Simon. 1973. The mind’s eye in chess. In Visual information-processing, ed. W. G. Chase. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. 1978. Knowledge structure and memory development. In Children s thinking: What develops?, ed. R. S. Siegler. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M., and S. Scribner. 1974. Culture and thought. New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M., J. Gay, J. Glick, and D. W. Sharp. 1971. The cultural context of learning and thinking. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, L. A. 1982. Strategies for visual comparison and representation: Individual differences. In vol. 1, Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, ed. R. J. Sternberg, 77–124. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, J. E., and R. J. Sternberg. 1984. The role of insight in intellectual giftedness. Gifted Child Quarterly 28:58–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeVet, C. V., and K. MacLean. 1958. Second game. New York: Street and Smith Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., and E. S. Elliott. 1983. Achievement motivation. In Handbook of child psychology, ed. P. H. Mussen, 643–91. New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feuerstein, R. 1980. Instrumental enrichment: An intervention program for cognitive modifiability. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. 1977. Cognitive development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, M. E. 1986. A living systems conceptualization of social intelligence: Outcomes, processes, and developmental change. In vol. 3, Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, ed. R. J. Sternberg, 119–71. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fry, P. S. 1984. Teachers‘ conceptions of students’ intelligence and intelligent functioning: A cross-sectional study of elementary, secondary, and tertiary level teachers. In Changing conceptions of intelligence and intellectual functioning: Current theory and research, ed. P. S. Fry, 157–74. New York: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galton, F. 1883. Inquiry into human faculty and its development. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. 1983. Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R., and R. Baillargeon. 1983. A review of some Piagetian concepts. In Handbook of child psychology. 4th ed., ed. P. H. Mussen. Vol. 3, Cognitive Development, ed. J. H. Flavell and E. M. Markman, 167–230. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R. S., and C. R. Gallistel. 1978. The child’s understanding of number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., and J. Grudin. 1985. The evolution of mental metaphors in psychology: A 90-year retrospective. American Psychologist 40:181–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gladwin, T. 1970. East is a big bird. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodnow, J. J. 1976. The nature of intelligent behavior: Questions raised by crosscultural studies. In The nature of intelligence, ed. L. B. Resnick. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregorc, T. 1985. Inside styles: Beyond the basics. Maynard, MA: Gabriel Systems, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P. 1967. The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gustafsson, J. E. 1984. A unifying model for the structure of intellectual abilities. Intelligence 8:279–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heath, S. S. 1983. Ways with words. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. L. 1973. Making vocational choices: A theory of careers. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J. L. 1968. Organization of abilities and the development of intelligence. Psychological Review 75:242–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J. L., and J. R. Knapp. 1973. On the subjective character of the empirical base of Guilford’s structure-of-intellect model. Psychological Bulletin 80:33–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E. B. 1978. Mechanics of verbal ability. Psychological Review 85:109–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E. B. 1980. Intelligence as an information-processing concept. British Journal of Psychology 71:449–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E. B., and M. Lansman. 1982. Individual differences in attention. In vol. 1, Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, ed. R. J. Sternberg. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E. B., M. Frost, and C. Lunneborg. 1973. Individual differences in cognition: A new approach to intelligence. In vol. 7, The psychology of learning and motivation, ed. G. Bower. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Intelligence and its measurement: A symposium. 1921. Journal of Educational Psychology 12:123–47, 195–216, 271–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irvine, S. H. 1969. Factor analysis of African abilities and attainments: Constructs across cultures. Psychological Bulletin 7:20–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irvine, S. H. 1979. The place of factor analysis in cross-cultural methodology and its contribution to cognitive theory. In Cross-cultural contributions to psychology, eds. L. Eckensberger and Y. Poortinga. Lisse, Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R. 1979. g: Outmoded theory or unconquered frontier? Creative Science and Technology 2:16–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R. 1980. Bias in mental testing. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R. 1982. Reaction time and psychometric g . in A model for intelligence, ed. H. J. Eysenck, 93–132. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jung, C. 1923. Psychological types. New York: Harcourt, Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J. 1976. Commentary on “Reflective and impulsive children: Strategies of information-processing underlying differences in problem solving.” Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 41, no. 5, serial no. 168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kearins, J. M. 1981. Visual spatial memory in Australian aboriginal children of desert regions. Cognitive Psychology 2:434–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keating, D. 1984. The emperor’s new clothes: The “new look” in intelligence research. In Vol. 2, Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, ed. R. J. Sternberg, 1–45. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. 1984. Mechanisms of cognitive development and the structure of knowledge. In Mechanisms of cognitive development, ed. R. J. Sternberg. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kogan, N. 1983. Stylistic variation in childhood and adolescence: Creativity, metaphor, and cognitive styles. In Handbook of child psychology. 4th ed., ed. P. H. Mussen. Vol. 3, Cognitive development, ed. J. H. Flavell and E. M. Markman, 630–706. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. 1980. Image and mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition. 1982. Culture and intelligence. In Handbook of human intelligence, ed. R. J. Sternberg, 642–719. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. 1976. Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M., E. B. Hunt, and N. N. Matthews. 1978. Individual differences in the verification of sentence-picture relationships. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 17:493–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maier, N. R. F., ed. 1970. Problem solving and creativity in individuals and groups. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R., and J. G. Greeno. 1972. Structural differences between learning outcomes produced by different instructional methods. Journal of Educational Psychology 63:165–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, R. P. 1974. Achieving school failure: An anthropological approach to illiteracy and social stratification. In Education and the cultural process, ed. G. Spindler. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milgram, S. 1975. Obedience to authority. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulholland, T. M., J. W. Pellegrino, and R. Glaser. 1980. Components of geometric analogy solution. Cognitive Psychology 12:252–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, I. B. 1980. Gifts differing. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, I. B., and M. H. McCaulley. 1985. Manual: A guide to the development and use of the Myers-Briggs type indicator. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. 1983. Components of intelligence or steps in routine procedures? Cognition 15:189–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., and H. A. Simon. 1972. Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrino, J. W., and R. Glaser. 1979. Cognitive correlates and components in the analysis of individual differences. In Human intelligence: Perspectives on its theory and measurement, eds. R. J. Sternberg and D. K. Detterman, 61–88. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrino, J. W., and R. Glaser. 1980. Components of inductive reasoning. In Aptitude, learning, and instruction. Vol. 1, Cognitive process analyses of aptitude, ed. R. E. Snow, P.-A. Federico, and W. E. Montague, 177–217. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrino, J. W., and R. Kail, Jr. 1982. Process analyses of spatial aptitude. In Vol. 1, Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, ed. R. J. Sternberg, 311–66. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. 1972. The psychology of intelligence. Totowa, NJ: Littlefield, Adams.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raaheim, K. 1974. Problem solving and intelligence. Oslo: Universitetsforlaget.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renzulli, J. S., and L. H. Smith. 1978. Learning styles inventory. Mansfield Center, CT: Creative Learning Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, L. B., and R. Glaser. 1976. Problem solving and intelligence. In The nature of intelligence, ed. L. B. Resnick. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. 1980. Memory metaphors in cognitive psychology. Memory and Cognition 8:231–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. 1980. How much intelligence is there in artificial intelligence? Intelligence 4:1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C., and R. P. Abelson. 1977. Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. 1986. Unities of thinking across domains. In The development of intelligence: The 1984 Minnesota symposium on child psychology, ed. M. Perlmutter. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. 1976. Identifying basic abilities underlying intelligent performance of complex tasks. In The nature of intelligence, ed. L. B. Resnick, 65–98. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A., and S. Reed. 1976. Modeling strategy shifts in a problem-solving task. Cognitive Psychology 8:86–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, R. E. 1979. Theory and method for research on aptitude processes. In Human intelligence: Perspectives on its theory and measurement, eds. R. J. Steinberg and D. K. Detterman, 105–38. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, R. E. 1980. Aptitude processes. In Aptitude, learning, and instruction. Vol. 1, Cognitive process analyses of aptitude, ed. R. E. Snow, P.-A. Federico, and W. E. Montague, 27–63. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, R. E., and D. F. Lohman. 1984. Toward a theory of cognitive aptitude for learning from instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology 75:347–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, R. E., P. C. Kyllonen, and B. Marshalek. 1984. The topography of ability and learning correlations. In Vol. 2, Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, ed. R. J. Sternberg, 47–104. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spear, L. C., and R. J. Sternberg. 1987. An information-processing framework for understanding reading disability. In Vol. 2, Handbook for cognitive, social, and neuropsychological aspects of learning disabilities, ed. S. Ceci, 3–31. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spearman, C. 1923. The nature of “intelligence” and the principles of cognition. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spearman, C. 1927. The abilities of man. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1977. Intelligence, information-processing, and analogical reasoning: The componential analysis of human abilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1979. The nature of mental abilities. American Psychologist 34: 214–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1980. Sketch of a componential subtheory of human intelligence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:573–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1981. Intelligence and nonentrenchment. Journal of Educational Psychology 73:1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1982a. Natural, unnatural, and supernatural concepts. Cognitive Psychology 14:451–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1982b. Reasoning, problem solving, and intelligence. In Handbook of human intelligence, ed. R. J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1983. Components of human intelligence. Cognition 15:1–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1984. Toward a triarchic theory of human intelligence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7:269–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1985a. Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1985b. Human intelligence: The model is the message. Science 230:1111–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1985c. Implicit theories of intelligence, creativity, and wisdom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 49:607–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1986a. Intelligence applied: Understanding and increasing your intellectual skills. San Diego: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1986b. Intelligence is mental self-government. In What is intelligence? Contemporary viewpoints on its nature and definition, ed. R. J. Sternberg and D. K. Detterman, 141–48. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1987. The psychology of verbal comprehension. In Vol. 3, Advances in instructional psychology, ed. R. Glaser, 97–151. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1988. Mental self-government: A theory of intellectual styles and their development. Human Development 31:197–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., and J. E. Davidson. 1982. The mind of the puzzler. Psychology Today 16 (June): 37–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., and D. K. Detterman, eds. 1986. What is intelligence? Contemporary viewpoints on its nature and definition. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., and M. K. Gardner. 1982. A componential interpretation of the general factor in human intelligence. In A model for intelligence, ed. H. J. Eysenck, 231–54. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., and J. L. Ketron. 1982. Selection and implementation of strategies in reasoning by analogy. Journal of Educational Psychology 74:399–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., and J. S. Powell. 1983. Comprehending verbal comprehension. American Psychologist 38:878–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., and B. Rifkin. 1979. The development of analogical reasoning processes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 27:195–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., and J. Suben. 1986. The socialization of intelligence. In Perspectives on intellectual development. Vol. 19, Minnesota symposia on child psychology, ed. M. Perlmutter, 201–35. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., and R. K. Wagner, eds. 1986. Practical intelligence: Nature and origins of competence in the everyday world. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., and E. M. Weil. 1980. An aptitude-strategy interaction in linear syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Educational Psychology 72:226–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., B. E. Conway, J. L. Ketron, and M. Bernstein. 1981. People’s conceptions of intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 41:37–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strong, E. K., Jr., D. P. Campbell, and J. C. Hansen. 1985. Strong-Campbell Interest Inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suchmann, R. G., and T. Trabasso. 1966. Color and form preference in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 3:177–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Super, C., and S. Harkness. 1980. The infant’s niche in rural Kenya and metropolitan America. In Issues in cross-cultural research, ed. L. L. Adler. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L. L. 1938. Primary mental abilities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. 1984. Conceptualizing intelligence: From an internal static attribution to the study of the process structure of organism-environment relationships. In Changing conceptions of intelligence and intellectual functioning: Current theory and research, ed. P. S. Fry, 63–89. New York: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, P. E. 1970. Creativity. Harmondsworth, England: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, P. E. 1971. The structure of human abilities. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. 1978. Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., and R. J. Stemberg. 1985. Practical intelligence in real-world pursuits: The role of tacit knowledge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 49:436–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., and R. J. Sternberg. 1987. Executive control in reading comprehension. In Executive control processes in reading, ed. B. K. Britton and S. M. Glynn, 1–21. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallach, M. A., and N. Kogan. 1965. Modes of thinking in young children. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. 1950. The measurement of appraisal of adult intelligence. 4th ed. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wissler, C. 1901. The correlation of mental and physical tests. Psychological Review, Monograph Supplement 3, no. 6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witkin, H. A. 1978. Cognitive styles in personal and cultural adaptation: The 1977 Heinz Werner lectures. Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yussen, S. R., and P. T. Kane. 1983. Children’s ideas about intellectual ability. In The child’s construction of social inequality, ed. R. Leahy, 109–33. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zigler, E., D. Balla, and R. Hodapp. 1984. On the definition and classification of mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency 89:215–30.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1992 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Sternberg, R.J. (1992). CAT: A Program of Comprehensive Abilities Testing. In: Gifford, B.R., O’Connor, M.C. (eds) Changing Assessments. Evaluation in Education and Human Services, vol 30. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-2968-8_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-2968-8_6

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-94-010-5318-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-011-2968-8

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics