Skip to main content

Human Cognition and Intelligence Towards an Integrated Theoretical Perspective

  • Chapter
Intelligence and Cognition: Contemporary Frames of Reference

Part of the book series: NATO ASI Series ((ASID,volume 38))

  • 159 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter presents a selective review of theoretical developments and empirical research results bearing on the nature of human cognition and intelligence. An attempt is made to show how concepts of cognition and intelligence have, historically, formed focusses in quite separate universes of discourse in scientific psychology. Cognition became a focal point in experimental psychology, aimed at identifying universal laws of mental function. Intelligence became the central concept in the study of individual differences in mental competence, their causes, correlates, and consequences. Only during the latter half of the last decade have psychologists begun to make a concerted effort to integrate knowledge and theory from the two formerly separate sub-disciplines. The purpose of this report is to appraise the progress being made towards this end and to highlight implications and opportunities for new research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Adkins, D.C., and Lyerly, S.B. (1952). Factor analysis of reasoning tests. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina,

    Google Scholar 

  • Anastasi, A. (1958). Differential Psychology. (3rd ed.) New York: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anastasi, A. (1972). Four hypotheses with a dearth of data: Response to Lehrke’s “A theory of X-linkage of major intellectual traits”. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 76, 620–622.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, A.L. (1951). The effect of laterality localization on focal brain lesions on the Wechsler-Bellevue subtests. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 7, 149–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J.E. (1939). The limitation of infant and preschool tests in the measurement of intelligence. Journal of Psychology, 8, 351–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J.R., and Reder, L.M. (1974). Negative judgements in and about semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 664–681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, D.M. (1976). A materialist theory of the mind. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R., and Shiffrin, R.M. (1968). Human memory: Proposed system and its control processes. In K.H. Spence and J.T. Spence (Eds.) The psychology of learning and motivation. Advances in research and theory, Vol. 2. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balinsky, B. (1974). An analysis of the mental factors of various age groups from nine to sixty. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 23, 191–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P.S., Cornelius, S.W., Spiro, A., Nesselroade, J.R., and Willis, S.L. (1980). Integration versus differentiation of fluid/crystallized intelligence in old age. Developmental Psychology, 16, 625–635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P.B., and Nesselroade, J.R. (1973). The developmental analysis of individual differences on multiple measures. In J.R. Nesselroade and H. W. Reese (Eds.) Life-span developmental psychology: Methodological issues. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P.B., and Schaie, X.W. (1976). On the plasticity of intelligence in adulthood and old age: Where Horn and Donaldson fail. American Psychologist, 31, 720–723.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barratt, G.V., Alexander, R.A., Doverspike, D., Cellar, D., and Thomas, G.C. (1982). The development and application of a computerized information processing test battery. Applied Psychological Measurement.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayley, N. (1943). Mental growth during the first three years. In R.G. Barker, J.S. Kounin, and H.F. Wright (Eds.) Child Behaviour and Development, New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bern, S.L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, J.R., MacDonald, G. S., Brace, S.M., and Menoyama, K. (1971). Some statistical properties of the visual evoked potential in man and their application as a criterion of normality. IEEE Transactions on Bio-medical Engineering, BME-18, 23–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berry, J.W. (1966). Temne and Eskimo perceptual skills. International Journal of Psychology, 1, 207–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berry, J.W. (1969). On cross-cultural comparability. International Journal of Psychology, 4, 199–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry, J.W. (1976). Human ecology and cognitive style: comparative studies in cultural and psychological adaptation. Beverly Hills: Sage/Halstead.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry, J.W. (1980). Introduction to methodology. In H.C. Triandis and J.W. Berry (Eds.) Handbook of Cross-cultural Psychology: Methodology (Volume 2). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berry, J.W., and Dasen, P. (Eds.) (1974). Introduction to culture and cognition. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biesheuvel, S. (1958). Objectives and methods of African psychological research. Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 161–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Binet, A., and Simon, Th. (1916). The development of intelligence in children. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkens.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binet, A., and Simon, Th. (1961). The development of intelligence in children. L’Annee Psychologique, 1905, pp. 163–191. Translated and reprinted in T. Shipley (Ed.) Classics in Psychology. New York: Philosophical Library, pp. 319–872.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, R. D., and Kolakowski, D. (1973). Further evidence of sex-linked major gene influence on human spatial ability. American Journal of Human Genetics, 25, 1–14.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bogen, J.E. (1970). Drugs and cerebral function. Springfield, Ill.: Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogen, J.E., Fisher, E.D., and Vogel, P.J. (1965). Cerebral commissurotomy: A second case report. Journal of the American Medical Assocation, 194, 1328–1329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brand, C. (1979). General intelligence and mental speed: their relationship and development. Paper read at NATO International Conference on Intelligence and Learning. York University, July.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brazier, M.A. (1962). The analysis of brain waves. Scientific American, 206. 142–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bierley, H. (1961). The speed and accuracy characteristics of neurotics. British Journal of Psychology, 52, 273–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brimer, M.A. (1969). Sex differences in listening comprehension. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 3, 72–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brislin, R. (1976). Comparative research methodology: cross-cultural studies. International Journal of Psychology, 11, 215–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brislin, R., Lonner, W.J., and Thorndike, R. (1973). Cross-cultural research methods. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broca, P. (1865). Du siege de la faculte de language articule. Bulletin de la Societe Anthropologie, 6, 377–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brody, S.B., and Brody, N. (1976). Intelligence: Nature, determinants, and consequences. New York; Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broverman, D.M., Klaiber, S.L., Kobayashy, G., and Vogel, W. (1968). The role of activation and inhibition in sex differences in cognitive abilities. Psychological Review, 57, 23–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Browne, M.W.. (1981). Covariance structures. In D.M. Hawkins (Ed.) Topics in applied multivariate analysis, Vol.2. Technical Report TWISK 193, Pretoria: National Institute for Mathematical Sciences, January.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J.S. (1957). Contemporary approaches to cognition. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J.S. (1964). The course of cognitive growth. American Psychologist, 19, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J.S., Goodnow, J.J., and Austin, G.A. (1956). A study of thinking. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryden, M.P. (1979). Evidence for sex-related differences in cerebral organization. In H.A. Wittig and A.C. Petersen (Eds.) Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Developmental issues. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buffery, A.W.H., and Gray, J.H. (1972). Sex differences in the development of spatial and linguistic skills. In C. Ounsted and D.C. Taylor (Eds.) Gender differences: theory, ontogeny and significance. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkens.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C. (1909). The experimental study of general intelligence. Child Study, 4, 78–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C. (1912). The inheritance of mental characters. Eugenics Review, 4, 1–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C. (1914). The measurement of intelligence by the Binet tests. Eugenics Review, 6, 36.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C. (1919). The development of reasoning in children. Journal of Experimental Prediatrics, 5, 68–77; 121–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C. (1938). The analysis of temperament. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 17, 158–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C. (1939). The factorial analysis of emotional traits. Character and Personality, 7, 238–254; 285–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C. (1940). The factors of the mind: An introduction to factor analysis in psychology. London: University of London Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C., (1945). The assessment of personality. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 15, 107–121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C. (1949). The structure of the mind: a review of the results of factor analysis. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 19, 176–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C. (1954). The bearing of the factor theory on the orgnization of schools and classes. Unpublished Report to the London County Council, London, 1919. (Cited in Burt, C., The differentiation of intellectual ability. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 24, 76–90).

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C. (1955). The evidence for the concept of intelligence. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 25, 153–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C. (1962). Mental and scholastic tests. (4th ed.) London: Staples.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C. (1963). Is intelligence distributed normally? British Journal of Statistical Psychology, 16, 175–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C. (1966). The genetic determination of differences in intelligence: a study of monozygotic twins reared together and apart. British Journal of Psychology, 57, 137–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, C. (1975). The gifted child. London: Hodder and Stoughton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, A., and Royce, J.R. (1975). Detecting cross-cultural commonalities and differences: intergroup factor analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 128–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butcher, G. (1969). Creativity in multivariate personality research. In R.M. Dreyer (Ed.) Contribution to the understanding of personality in honour of R.3. Cattell. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calloway, E. (1973). Correlation between averaged evoked potentials and measures of intelligence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 29, 553–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J.B. (1974). Psychometric tests as cognitive tasks: A new “structure-of-intellect”. RB 74–16, Technical Report Ho. 4, Princeton: Educational Testing Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J.B. (1978a). How shall we study individual differences in cognitive abilities? Methodological and theoretical perspectives. Intelligence, 1, 2, 87–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J.B. (1978b). Book review: “Intelligence, information processing and analogical reasoning: The componential analysis of human abilities” by Robert J. Sternberg. Hillsdale, N.J.: Eribaum. In Applied Psychological Measurement, 2 (4), 615–618.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J.B. (1980). Individual difference relations in psychometric and experimental cognitive tasks. Report No. 163, AD A086057. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J.B. (1981). Ability and task difficulty in cognitive psychology. Educational Researcher, 10, 11–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J.B., and Maxwell, S.E. (1979). Individual differences in human cognitive abilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 30, 603–640.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carter-Saltzman, L. (1979). Patterns of cognitive functioning in relation to handedness and sex-related differences. In H.A. Wittig and A.C. Petersen (Eds.) Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Developmental issues. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, J.McK. (1890). Mental tests and measurements. Mind, 15, 373–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, JMcK. and Ferrand, L. (1896). Physical and mental measurements of the students of Columbia University. Psychological Review, 3, 618–648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R.B. (1941). Some theoretical issues in adult intelligence testing. Psychological Bulletin, 38, 592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R.B. (1943). The measurement of adult intelligence. Psychological Bulletin. 40, 153–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R.B. (1957). The IPAR Culture Fair Intelligence Scales. Champaign, Ill.: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R.B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R.B. (1967). The theory of fluid and crysallized general intelligence checked at the 5–6 year-old level. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 37, 209–224.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R.B. (1971). Abilities; their structure, growth, and action. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R.B., and Beloff, H. (1959). The high school personality questionnaire. Champaign, Ill.: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R.B., Blewett, D.B., and Beloff, J.R. (1955). The inheritance of personality. A multiple variance analysis determination of approximate nature-nurture ratios for primary personality factors in Q-data. American Journal of Human Genetics, 7, 122–146.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chalke, F.C.R., and Ertl, J.P. (1965). Evoked potentials and intelligence. Life Sciences, 4, 1319–1322.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H.H., and Chase, W.G. (1972). On the process of comparing sentences against pictures. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 472–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cleary, T.A., Linn, R.L., and Rock, D.A. (1968). An exploratory study of programmed tests. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 28, 345–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, S. (1965). Brain size. Archives of Neurology, 1965, 12, 555–561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, D., and Wilkie, F. (1979). Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Developmental issues. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, L.A., and Podgorny, P. (1976). Mental transformations of visual comparison processes: Effects of complexity and similarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 503–514.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, L.A., and Shepard, R.N. (1973). Chromometric studies of the rotation of mental images. In V.G. Chase (ed.) Visual Information Processing. New York: Academic Press, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cory, C.H., Rimland, B., and Bryson, R.A. (1977). Using computerized tests to measure new dimensions of abilities: An exploratory study. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 101–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, C.B. A canonical correlation analysis of cortical evoked response and intelligence test data? Canadian Journal of Psychology, 28, 319–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crawford-Nutt, D.H. (1977). The assessment, of mental ability among Black teachers in Bophuthatswana. CSIR Confidential Contract Report C/PERS 257. Johannesburg: National Institute for Personnel Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L.J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 12, 671–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L.J. (1967). Year-to-year correlations of mental tests: A review of the Hofstatter analysis. Child Development, 38, 283–289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W.R. (1980). Age comparative factor analysis of ability variables in adulthood and old age. Intelligence, 4, 133–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W.R. (1981). Ability factor structure differences in adulthood and old age. Multivariate Behavioural Research, 16, 3–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W.R., and Birren, G.E. (1980). Age changes in the factor structure of intellectual abilities in adulthood and old age. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 40, 271–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dan, A.G. (1979). The menstrual cycle and sex-related differences in cognitive variability. In H.A. Wittig and A.C. Petersen (Eds.) Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Developmental issues. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1859). The origin of species. London: Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dasen, P.R. (1977). Are cognitive processes universal? A contribution to cross-cultural Piagetian psychology. In N. Warren (Ed.) Studies in cross-cultural psychology, Volume 1. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, J.L.M. (1969). Theoretical and research bases of bio-social psychology. University of Hong Kong Gazette, 16, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, J.L.M. (1971). Theory and research in cross-cultural psychology. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 24, 291–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Desmedt, J.E. (Ed.) (1979). Cognitive components in cerebral event-related potentials and selective attention. Basel: Karger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dillon, R.F. (1981). Analogical reasoning under different methods of test administration. Applied Psychological Measurement, 5, 341–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dimond, S.G. (1980). Neuropsychology:, A text book of systems and psychological functions of the human brain. London: Butterworths.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donchin, E. (1966). A multivariate approach to the analysis of average evoked potentials. IEEE Transactions on bio-medical engineering, BME-13, 131–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donchin, E., Calloway, E., and Jones, R.T. (1970). Auditory evoked potential variables in schizophrenia, II. The application of discriminant analysis. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 29, 429–440.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donchin, E., and Lindsley, D.B. (Eds.) (1969). Average evoked potentials. Washington: NASA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dorfman, D.D. (1978). The Cyril Burt question: New findings. Science, 201, No. 4362, 117–1186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drenth, P.J.D., Van der Vlier, H., and Omari, I.M. (1979). The use of classroom tests, examinations and aptitude tests in a developing country. In L. Ekensberger, W. Lonner, and Y.H. Poortinga (Eds.) Cross-cultural contributions to Psychology. Lisse, Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunham, G.L., and Bunderson, C.V. (1969). Effect of decision-rule instruction upon the relationship of cognitive abilities to performance in multiple-category concept problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 60, 121–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egan, D.E. (1976). Accuracy and latency scores as measures of spatial information processing. Research Report No. 1224-Pensacola, Florida: Naval Aerospace Medical Research Laboratory, February.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egan, D.S. (1978). Characterizing spatial ability: Different mental processes reflected in accuracy and latency scores. Research Report No. 1250. Pensacola, Florida: Naval Aerospace Medical Research Laboratory.

    Google Scholar 

  • El Koussi, A.A.H. (1935). The visual perception of space. British Journal of Psychology, Monograph Supplement, No. 20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekensberger, L. (1972). The necessity of a theory for applied cross-cultural research. In L.J.C. Cronbach and P.J.D. Drenth (Eds.) Mental tests and cultural adaptation. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elkind, D. and Flavell, J.H. (Eds.) (1969). Studies in cognitive development: Essays in honour of Jean Piaget. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellingson, R.J. (1965). Relationship between EEG and test intelligence: A commentary. Psychological Bulletin, 65, 91–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, H. (1894). Han and Woman; A study of human secondary sexual characters. (4th ed.) London: Walter Scott.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elwood, D.L. (1969). Automation of psychological testing. American Psychologist, 287–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ertl, J.P. (1968). Evoked potentials, neural efficiency, and I.Q. Paper presented at the International Symposium for Biocybernetics, Washington, D.C.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ertl, J.P. (1972) I.Q. evoked responses and Fourier analysis. Nature, 241, 209–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ertl, J.P., and Schafer, E.W.P. (1969). Brain response correlates of psychometric intelligence. Nature, 223, 421–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H.J. (1953). Uses and abuses of psychology. London: Pelican.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H.J. (1967). Intelligence assessment: A theoretical and experimental approach. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 37, 81–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fairweather, H. (1976). Sex differences in cognition. Cognition, 4, 231–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, G.A. (1954). On learning and human ability. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 8, 95–112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, G.A. (1956). On transfer and the abilities of man. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 10, 121–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • French, J.W. (1951). The description of aptitude and achievement tests in terms of rotated factors. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N.H., and Jahoda, G. (1966). On the scope and methods of cross-cultural research. International Journal of Psychology, 1, 110–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Furneaux, W.D. (1960). Intellectual abilities and problem-solving behaviour. In H.J. Eysenck (Ed.) Handbook of Abnormal Psychology, Chapter 5. London: Pitman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gale, A., Brown, A., Osborne, K., and Smallbone, A. (1978). Further evidence of sex differences in brain organization. Biological Psychology, 203–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galton, F. (1869). Hereditary genius. London: MacMillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Galton, F. (1883). Enquiries into human faculty and its development. London: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garnett, J.C.M. (1919). On certain independent factors in mental measurement. Proceedings of the Royal Society, 26, 102–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, H.E. (1946). A developmental theory of intelligence. American Psychologist, 1, 372–378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M.S., and Le Doux, J.E. (1978). The integrated mind. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N., Galaburda, A., and Le May, M. (1979). Morphological and physiological substrates of language and cognitive development. In R. Katzman (Ed.) Congenital and acquired cognitive disorders. New York: Raven.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, R., and Pellegrino, J.W. (1978). Uniting cognitive process theory and differential psychology: Back home from the wars. Intelligence, 2, 305–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glees, P. (1967). Are both sides of our central nervous system of equal value? Triangle, The Sandoc Journal of Medical Science, 8, 101–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S.J. (1978). Morton’s ranking of races by cranial capacity: unconscious manipulation of data may be a scientific norm. Science, 200, 503–509.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grady, K.E. (1977). The belief in sex differences. Paper presented at Eastern Psychological Association Meeting, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwood, D.I., and Taylor, C. (1965). Adaptive testing in an older population. Journal of Psychology, 60, 193–198.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J.P. (1947). Printed classification tests. A.A.F. Aviation Psychology Report Ho. 5, Washington, D.C.: Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J.P. (1956). The structure of intellect. Psychological Bulletin, 53, 267–293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J.P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guildford, J.P. (1979). Cognitive Psychology with a frame of reference. San Diego: Edits.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J.P. (1980). Fluid and crystallized intelligence: two fanciful concepts. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 406–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J.P., and Hoepfner, R. (1971). The analysis of intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J.P., and Tenopyr, M.L. (1968). Implications of the structure of intellect model for high school and college students. In W.M. Michael (Ed.) Teachings for creative endeavour. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, L. (1950). Relation of scalogram analysis to other techniques. In S.A. Stoupfer et al., (Eds.) Measurement and prediction. New Jersey: Princeton University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, L. (1954). The radex: A new apprach to factor analysis. In P. Lazarsfeld (Ed.) Mathematical thinking in the social sciences. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, L. (1955). A generalized simplex for factor analysis. Psychometrika, 20, 173–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, L. (1965a). A faceted definition of intelligence. In R. Eiferman (Ed.) Scripta Hierosolymitana studies in pscyhology. Jerusalem: The Hebrew University, 14, 166–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, L. (1965b). The structure of interrrelations among intelligence tests. In Proceedings of the 1964 Invitational Conference on Testing Problems. Princeton, New Jersey: Educational Testing Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, L. (1967). The development of non-metric space analysis: A letter to Professor John Ross. Multivariate Behavioural Research, 2, 71–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, L. (1968). A general non-metric technique for finding the smallest coordinate space for a configuration of points. Psychometrika, 33, 469–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, L., and Schlesinger, I.M. (1966). Systematic construction of distractors for ability and achievement test items. Educational and Psychological Measurement.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hakstian, A.R., and Vandenberg, S.G. (1979). The cross-cultural generalizability of a higher-order cognitive structure model. Intelligence, 3, 73–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, J.A. (1936). Intelligence and the human brain. Psychological Review, 43, 308–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H.F. (1949). The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 56, 51–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, L.J. (1978). Sex differences in spatial ability: possible environmental, genetic, and neurological factors. In M. Kinsbourne (Ed.) Asymmetrical function of the brain. Hew York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hearnshaw, L.S. (1979). Cyril Burt: Psychologist. London: Hodder and Stoughton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hearnshaw, L.S. (1964). A short history of 3ritish psychology. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D.O. (1942). The effects of early and late brain injury upon test scores and the nature of normal adult intelligence. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 89, 275–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendrickson, A.E. (1982) The biological basis of intelligence. Part I: Theory. London: Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendrickson, D.S. (1981). The biological basis of intelligence. Part II: Measurement. London: Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendrickson, D.S., and Hendrickson, A.E. (1980). The biological basis of individual differences in intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 1, 3–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hick, W. (1952). On the rates of gain of information. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 4, 11–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, D. (1981). Mechanisms of the mind: a psychiatric perspective. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 54, 1–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, J.P. (1967). Similarity and accordance between parents and sons in attitudes towards mathematics. Child Development, 38, 777–791.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J. R. (1947). Sex differences in primary mental abilities. Journal of Educational Research, 41, 126–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoepfner, R., Guilford, J.P., and Bradley, P.A. (1970). Information-transformation abilities. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 30, 785–802.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofstatter, P.R. (1954). The changing composition of intelligence. A study in T-technique. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 85, 159–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holzinger, K.J. (1937). Student manual of factor analysis. Chicago: Department of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J.L. (1967). On subjectivity in factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 27, 811–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J.L. (1968). Organization of abilities and the development of intelligence. Psychological Review, 75, 242–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J.L. (1970). Organization of data on life-span development of human abilities. In L.R. Goulet and B.P. Baltes (Eds.) Life-span Development Psychology. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J.L. (1971). Integration of concepts of reliability and standard error of measurement. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 31, 57–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J.L. (1976a). Human abilities: A review of research and theory in the early 1970’s. Annual Review of Psychology, 27, 437–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J.L. (1976b). Progress report on a study of speed, power, carefulness, and short-term learning components of intelligence and changes in these components in adulthood. Army Research Institue, Grant No. DAHC 19/74–5-0012. Denver: University of Denver.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J.L. (1978). Human ability systems. Life-span Development and Behaviour, 1, 211–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J.L., and Bramble, W.G. (1967). Second-order ability structure revealed in rights and wrongs scores. Journal of Educational Psychology, 58, 115–122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J.L., and Cattell, R.B. (1966). Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallised general intelligence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 57, 253–270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J.L., and Donaldson, G. (1976). On the myth of intellectual decline in adulthood. American Psychologist, 31, 701–709.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J.L., Donaldson, G., and Engstrom, R. (1981). Apprehension, memory, and fluid intelligence decline in adulthood. Research on Aging, 3, 33–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J.L., and Knapp, J.R. (1973). On the subjective character of the empirical base of Guilford’s structure-of-intellect model. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 33–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J.L., and McArdle, J.J. (1980). Perspectives on mathematical/statistical model building (MASMOD) in research on aging. In L.W. Poon (Ed.) Aging in the 80’s: Psychological issues. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J.L., and Stankov, L. (1981). Auditory and visual factors of intelligence. Unpublished Research Report, University of Denver, Denver, Colorado.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, L.G. (1960). Investigations of the simplex. Psychometrika, 25, 213–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, L.G.(1961). The organization of human abilities. Presidential Address, Division 5 of American Psychological Association. New York: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, L.G. (1962). The organization of human abilities. American Psychologist, 17, 475–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E. (1971). What kind of computer is man? Cognitive Psychology, 2, 52–98..

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E. (1978). Qualitative sources of individual differences in complex problem-solving. In J.M. Scandura and C.J. Brainerd (Eds.) Structural/process models of complex human behaviour. The Netherlands: Sitjhoff and Noordhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E. (1979). Intelligence as an information processing concept. Research Report 04206. Department of Psychology, University of Washington: Seattle, Washington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E., Forst, N., and Lunneborg, C. (1973). Individual differences in cognition: A new approach to intelligence. In G. Bower (Ed.) The psychology of learning and motivation, (Vol.7). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E., Lunneborg, C.E., and Lewis, G. (1975). What does it mean to be high verbal? Cognitive Psychology, 7, 194–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, J. McV. (1961). Intelligence and Experience. New York: Ronald.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyman, R. (1953). Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, 188–190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Irvine, S.H. (1966). Towards a rationale for testing attainments and abilities in Africans. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 36, 24–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Irvine, S.H. The place of factor analysis in cross-cultural methodology and its contribution to cognitive theory. In L. Eckensberger, I. Poortinga and W. Lonner (Eds.), Cross-cultural Contributions to Psychology. Lisse: Swets and Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irvine, S.H., and Carroll, W.K. (1980). Testing and assessment across cultures: Issues in methodology and theory. In K.C. Triandis and J.W. Berry (Eds.) Handbook of Cross-cultural Psychology. Volume 2: Methodology. Boston, Massachusetts: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, G.H. (1887). Remarks on evolution and dissolution of the nervous system. Journal of Medical Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, E. (1973). Electrophysiology of mental activities and introduction to the psychological process of thinking. In F.G. McGuigan and H.A. Schoonover (Eds.) The psychophysiologic of thinking: Studies of correct procedures. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jahoda, G.A. (1970). A cross-cultural perspective in psychology. The Advancement of Science, 27, 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A.R. (1967). Estimation of the limits of heritability of traits by comparison of monozygotic and dizygotic twins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 149–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A.R. (1973). Level I and Level II abilities in three ethnic groups. American Educational Journal, 4, 263–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A.R. (1977). The problem of genotype-environment correlation and the estimation of heritability from monozygotic and dizygotic twins. Acta Geneticae, Medicae and Gemellologiae.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A.R. (1979). g — Outmoded theory or unconquered frontier? Creative Science and Technology, 2, 16–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kearsley, G. and Royce, J.R. (1977). A multi-factor theory of sensation: individuality in sensory structure and sensory processing. Perceptual and Motor Skill, 44, 1299–1316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, T.L. (1923). Statistical Method. New York: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, T.L. (1928). Cross roads in the mind of man. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, T.L. (1935). Essential traits of mental life. Harvard: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kingsbury, G., and Weiss, D.J. (1980). A comparison of adaptive, sequential, and conventional testing strategies for mastery decisions. Research Report 80–4, AD A094478, Minneapolis: University of Minnesota.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kline, P. (1979). Psychometrics and Psychology. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koestler, A. (1979). Janus: A summing up. London: Pan Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of gestalt psychology. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, W. (1929). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York: Liveright, 1947.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kornhuber, H.H. (1978). A reconsideration of the mind-brain problem. In P.A. Buser and A. Rougeuel-Buser (Eds.) Cerebral correlates of conscious experience. Inserm. Symposium No.6. Amsterdam: North Holland Biomedical Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. (1950). In search of the engram. Symposium of the Society of Experimental Biology, 4, 454–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrke, R.G. (1974). X-linked mental retardation and verbal disability. In The National Foundation — March of Dimes, Birth Defects Original Article Series, 10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrke, R.G. (1978). Sex linkage: A biological basis for greater male variability in intelligence. In R.T. Osborne, C.E. Noble, and N. Weyl (Eds.) Human variation: the biopsychology of age, race, and sex. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, B.M. (1961). Sub-cultural values and I.Q. stability. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 98, 69–82.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, B.M. (1963). The W.A.I.S. quotient of sub-cultural deviation. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 100, 103–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, J. (1971). Lateral specialization of the human brain: Behavioural manifestation and possible evolutionary basis. 32nd Annual Biology Colloquium on the biology of behaviour. Corrali, Oregon: Oregon State University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, J. (1974). Psychobiological implications of bilateral asymmetry. In S.J. Dimond and J.G. Beaumont (Eds.) Hemisphere function in the human brain. London: Elek. Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, J. (1978). Lateral differences in the human brain in cognition and behavioural control. In P.A. Buser and A. Rougeuel-Buser (Eds.) Cerebral correlates of conscious experience. Inserm. Symposium No. 6. Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lijphart, A. (1971). Comparative politics and comparative method. American Political Science Review, 65, 682–693.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsley, D.B. (1944). Electroencephalography. In J. McV. Hunt (Ed.) Personality and the behaviour disorders, Vol.2. New York: Ronald Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lonner, W.J. (1980). Introduction to basic processes. In H.C. Triandis and W.J. Lonner (Eds.) Handbook of Cross-cultural psychology: Basic processes (Volume 3). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, F.M. (1969). Estimating true score distributons in psychological testing. Psychometrika, 34, 259–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, P.M. (1971). The self-scoring flexilevel test. Journal of Educational Measurement, 8, 14. 7–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, F.M. (1977). A broad-range tailored test of verbal ability. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 95–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E.E., and Jacklin, C.N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E.E., and Jacklin, C.N. (1975). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matarazzo, J.D. (1976). Wechsler’s measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence (5th ed.). Baltimore: Williams and Wilkens.

    Google Scholar 

  • McArdle, J.J., and Orn, J.L. (1981). Structural equation models of Gf-Gc Intelligence theory. Paper presented at American Psychological Association Annual Meeting. Los Angeles.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDougal, W. (1932). Energies of men. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGee, M.G. (1979). Human spatial abilities: sources of sex differences. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • McMemar, Q. (1964). Lost: our intelligence. Why? American Psychologist, 19, 871–832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metzler, J., and Shepard, R.N. (1974). Transformational studies of the internal representation of three dimensional objects. In R.L. Solso (Ed.) Theories in cognitive psychology; the Loyola symposium. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michael, W.B. (Ed.) (1968). Teaching for creative endeavour. Bloomington: Indiana Univesity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, L. (1978). Has artificial intelligence contributed to an understanding of the human mind? A critique of arguments for and against. Cognitive Science, 2, 111–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Money, J., and Ehrhardt, A. (1972). Man and woman, boy and girl. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mountcastle, V.B. (1961). Outer hemispheric relations and cerebral dominance. Baltimore, Maryland: Johns Hopkins Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nash, S.C. (1979). Sex role as a mediator of intellectual functioning. In M.A. Wittig and A.C. Peterson (Eds.) Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Developmental issues. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesselroade, J.R. (1970). Application of multivariate strategies to problems of structuring long-term change. In L.R. Goulet and P.B. Baltes (Eds.) Life-span developmental psychology: Research and theory. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mettelbeck, T. and Lally, M. (1976). Inspection time and measured intelligence. British Journal of Psychology, 67, 17–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. (1973). Production systems and control processes. In W.G. Chase (Ed.) Visual information processing. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D.A. (1979). Twelve issues for cognitive science. Technical Report AD 80992. Centre for Human Information Processing, University of California: San Diego, La Jolla.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, J. (1943). Structural vizualization. Boston: Human Engineering Laboratory.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ostow, M. (1950). Psychic function and the electroencephalogram. Archives of Neurological Psychiatry, 64, 385–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pawlik, K. (1966). Concepts in human cognition and aptitudes. In R.B. Cattell (Ed.) Handbook of multivariate experimental psychology. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, K. (1902). On the influence of natural selection on the variability and correlation of organs. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc, 200, 1–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pemberton, C. (1952). The closure factors related to other cognitive processes. Psychometrika, 17, 267–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penfield, W. M. and Perot, P. (1963). The brain’s record of auditory and visual experience. Brain, 86, 595–695.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Penfield, W., and Rasmussen, T. (1950). The cerebral cortex of man: A clinical study of the localization of functions. New York: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, N.W., McCoy, J.G., Cunningham, W.R., Falgout, J.C., and Street, W.J. (1976). Multivariate visual evoked response correlates of intelligence. Psychophysiology, 13, 323–329.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, A.C. (1976). A physical androgyny of cognitive functioning in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 12, 524–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, A.C. (1979). Hormones and cognitive functioning in normal development. In M.A. Wittig and A.C. Petersen (Ms.) Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Developmental issues. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, A.C., and Wittig, M.A. (1979). Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: an overview. In M.A. Wittig and A.C. Petersen (Eds.) Sex-related issues in cognitive functioning: Developmental issues. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1936). The origin of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1946). The psychology of intelligence. London: Routledge and Kegan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1964). The early growth of logic in the child. Translated by E.A. Lunzer and D. Papert. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plum, A. (1968). Visual evoked responses: their relationship to intelligence. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poortinga, Y. (1971). Cross-cultural comparison of maximum performance tests: Some methodological aspects and some experiments with simple auditory and visual stimuli. Psychologia Africana Monograph Supplement, 6, 1–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poortinga, I. (1975a). Some implications of three different approaches to intercultural comparison. In J.W. Berry and W.J. Lonner (Eds.) Applied cross-cultural psychology. Amsterdam: Swetz and Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poortinga, Y. (1975b). Limitations on international comparison of psychological data. Netherlandse Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie, 30, 23–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Popper, K.R., and Eccles, J.C. (1977). The self and its brain. New York: Springer International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M.I. (1978). Chronometric explorations of mind. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, A., Katzko, M., and Royce, J.R. (1978). A multi-factor-systen theory of the structure and dynamics of motor function. Journal of Motor Behaviour. 10, 191–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preston, R.C. (1962).Reading achievement of German and American children. School and Society, 90, 350–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, R.L., and Smith, D.B.D. (1974). The P300 wave of the averaged evoked potential. A bibliography. Physiological Psychology, 2, 381–391.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pucetti, R. (1977). Bilateral organization of consciousness. In S.J. Dimond and D.A. Blizard (Ms.) Evolution and lateralization of the brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 229, 448–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinert, G. (1970). Comparative factor analytic studies of intelligence throughout the human life-span. In L.R. Goulet and P.B. Baltes (Eds.) Life-span developmental psychology: Research and theory. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, L.E., Dustman, R.E., and Beck, E.C. (1969). The visual evoked response: A comparison of bright and dull children. EEG Clinical Neuropsychology, 27, 364–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richards, L.G., and Platnick, D.M. (1974). Word-recognition thresholds as a function of word length. American Journal of Psychology, 87, 65–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, E. (1964). Die Geschwindigkeit der Verarbeitung vond Information und ihr Zusammenhang met Intelligenz. Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 11, 616–622.

    Google Scholar 

  • Royce, J.R. (1980). Factor analysis is alive and well. American Psychologist. 35, 390–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Royce, J. R. (1977). Genetics, environment and intelligence: A theoretical synthesis. In A. Oliverio (Ed.) Genetics, environnent and intelligence. Elsevier: North Holland Biomedical Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Royce, J.R., and Buss, A.R. (1977). The role of general systems and information theory in multi-factor individuality theory. Canadian Psychological Review, 17, 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joyce, J.R. (1979). Toward a viable theory of individual differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1927–1931.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Royce, J.R., and McDermott, J. (1977). A multi-dimensional system dynamics model of affect. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 193–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samejima, F. (1977). A use of the information function in tailored testing. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 223–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarr-Calapatek, S. (1975). Genetics and the development of intelligence. In F.D. Horowitz (Ed.) Review of Child Development Research, Vol.4. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaie, K.W. (1970). A re-interpretation of age-related changes in cognitive structure and functioning. In L.R. Goulet and P.B. Baltes (Eds.) Life-span developmental psychology: Research and theory. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlopflocher, D.A., Royce, J.R., and Meehan, K. (1979). A multi-factor theory of values (Center Paper). Unpublished manuscript, University of Atlanta: Edmonton, Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, F.L., and Crano, W.D. (1974). A test of the theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence in middle- and low-socio-economic-status children: A cross-lagged panel analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 255–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidtke, H. (1961). Zur Frage der informations theoretischen Analyse von Wahreaktionsexperimenten. Psychologische Forschung, 26, 157–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, F.H.K., and Moir, W.L.N. (1980). Intelligence and semantic judgement time. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 1, 53–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R.N., and Feng, C.A. (1972). A Chronometric study of mental paper folding. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 228–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R.N., and Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, J.A. (1978). Sex-related cognitive differences: An essay on theory and evidence. Springfield, Illinois: Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shucard, C.W., and Horn, J.L. (1972). Evoked cortical potentials and measurement of human abilities. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 78, 59–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H.A. (1979). Information processing models of cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 30, 363–396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, I.M. (1964). Spatial ability. San Diego: Robert P. Knapp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spearman, C. (1904). “General Intelligence”, objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 201–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spearman, C. (1914). The theory of two factors. Psychological Review, 21, 101–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spearman, C. (1923). The nature of “intelligence” and the principles of cognition. London: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of Man: Their nature and measurement. London: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spearman, C., and Hart, B. (1912). General ability: Its existence and nature. British Journal of Psychology, 5, 51–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, H., (1855). The principles of psychology. London: Williams and Norgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperry, R.W. (1968a). Mental unity following surgical disconnection of the cerebral hemispheres. The Harvey Lectures, Series 62. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperry, R.W (1968). Hemisphere deconnection and unity in conscious awareness. American Psychologist, (b), 23, 723–733.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sperry, R.W. (1969). A modified concept of consciousness. Psychological Review, 76, 532–636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sperry, R.W. (1970). Cerebral dominance in perception. In F.A. Young and D.B. Lindsey (Eds.) Early experience in visual information processing in perceptual and reading disorder. Washington, D.C.: National Academy of Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperry, R.W. (1973). Lateral specialization of cerebral function in the surgically separated hemispheres. In F.J. McGuigan and R.A. Schoonover (Eds.) The psychophysiology of thinking. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperry, R.W., Gazzaniga, M.S., and Bogen, J.E. (1969). Interhemispheric relationships: The neocortical commissures: syndromes of hemisphere disconnection. In P.J. Vinken and G.W. Bruyn (Eds.) Handbook of clinical neurology, Vol. 4. Amsterdam: North Holland, 273–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stankov, L., and Horn, J.L. (1980). Human abilities revealed through auditory tests. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, (1).

    Google Scholar 

  • Stankov, L., Horn, J.L., and Roy, T. (1980). On the relationship between Gf/Gc theory and Jensen’s Level I/Level II theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 796–809.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephenson, W. (1931). Tetrad differences for non-verbal subtests. Tetrad differences for verbal subtests relative to non-verbal subtests. Journal of Educational Psychology. 22, 167–185; 225–267; 334–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, C. (1960). Human Genetics. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, W. (1914; 1912). The psychological method of testing intelligence. Translated by G.M. Whipple. Baltimore: Warwick and York (1912).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R.J. (1977). Intelligence, information processing, and analogical reasoning: The componential analysis of human abilities. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R.J. (1978). Intelligence research at the interface between differential and cognitive psychology. Intelligence, 2, 195–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R.J. (1979). The nature of mental abilities. American Psychologist, 34, 214–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R.J. (1980). Claims, counterclaims, and components: A counter critique of componential analysis. Technical Report No. 25 AD 8086797. New Haven: Department of Psychology, Yale University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S. (1969). Memory scanning: Mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. American Scientist, 57, 421–457.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Street, W.J., Perry, N.W., and Cunningham, W.R. (1976). A factor analysis of visual evoked responses. Psychophysiology, 13, 352–356.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J.R. (1938). Factors affecting speed in serial verbal reactions. Psychological Monographs, 50, (5), 38–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szymanski, A. (1980). Computer simulation of human thought — its perspectives and constraints. Kybernetics, 9, 9–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomson, G.H. (1939). The factorial analysis of human ability. London: University of London Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E.L. (1909). The relation of accuracy in sensory discrimination to general intelligence. American Journal of Psychology, 20, 364–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E.L. (1926). The measurement of intelligence. New York: Teachers’ College, Columbia University.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L.L. (1924). The nature of intelligence. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L.L. (1926). The mental age concept. Psychological Review, 33, 268–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L.L. (1927). A unit of mental meausrement. Psychological Review, 34, 415–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L.L. (1931). Multiple factor analysis. Psychological Review, 38, 406–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L.L. (1933). The theory of multiple factors. Chicago: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L.L. (1935). The vectors of the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L.L. (1936). The factorial isolation of primary abilities. Psychometrika, 1, 175–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L.L. (1938). Primary mental abilities. Psychometric Monographs, No. 1. Chicago: University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L.L. (1940). Experimental study of simple structure. Psychometrika, 5, 153–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L.L. (1944). A factorial study of perception. Psychometric Monographs, 4, vi-148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L.L. (1947). Multiple factor analysis: A development and expansion of “Vectors of the Mind”. Chicago: University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L.L. (1950). Some primary abilities in visual thinking. Proceedings of the Americal Philosophical Society, 94, 517–521.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L.L., and Thurstone, T.G. (1941). Factorial studies of intelligence. Psychometric Monographs No.2. Chicago: University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H.C., Malpass, R.S., and Davison, A. (1972). Cross-cultural psychology. Biennial Review of Anthropology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, L.R. (1951). A method for synthesis of factor-analytic studies. PRS Report No. 984, Department of the Army, Adjutant General’s Office, Personnel Research Section: Washington, D.C.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, L.S. (1965). The psychology of human differences (3rd ed.). New York: Appleton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, L.E. (1978). Individuality, human possibilities and personal choice in the psychological development of men and women. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Undheim, J.O. (1976). Ability structure in 10–11 year-old Children and the theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 4.11–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Undheim, J.O. (1978). Broad ability factors in 12 to 13 year-old children and the theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence and the differentiation hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 433–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vandenberg, S.G. (1977). Hereditary abilities in man. In A. Oliverio (Ed.) Genetics, environment, and intelligence. Elsevier: North Holland Biomedical.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vandenberg, S.G., and Hakstian, A.R. (1978). Cultural influences on cognition: A reanalysis of Vernon’s data. International Journal of Psychology. 13, 251–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vandenberg, S.G., and Kuse, A.R. (1979). Spatial ability: A critical review of the sex-linked major gene hypothesis. In M.A. Wittig and A.C. Petersen (Eds.) Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Developmental issues. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van de Vijver, F.J.R., and Poortinga, I.H. (1982). Cross-cultural generalization and universality. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 13, 387–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varela, J.A. (1969). Elaboration of Guilford’s S-I model. Psychological Review, 76, 332–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, P.E. (1950). The structure of human abilities. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, P.E. (1961). The structure of human abilities (2nd ed.). London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, P.E. (1964). The psychology of intelligence and g. In J. Cohen (Ed.) Readings in psychology. London: Allen and Uwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, P.E. (1969a). Intelligence and cultural environment. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, P.E. (1969b). Cross-cultural applications of factor analysis. Proceedings of the 26th International Congress of Applied Psychology. Amsterdam: Swets and Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, P.E. (1979). Intelligence: Heredity and Environment. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verster, J.M. (1975). A dimension of conceptual speed. Psychologia Africana, 16, 45–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verster, J.M. (1982a). The structure, organization, and correlates of cognitive speed and accuracy: a cross-cultural study using computerized tests. In S.H. Irvine and J.W. Berry (Eds.) Human assessment and cultural factors. Hew York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verster, J.M. (1982b). A cross-cultural study of cognitive processes using computerized tests. Unpublished D. Litt. et Phil. thesis, Pretoria: University of South Africa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verster, J.M. Evaluation of a structural model of cognitive processes: an empirical investigation in different populations using computerized tests. Johannesburg: National Institute for Personnel Research, in preparation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verster, J.M., and Steyn, D.W. (1973). The development of a high level symbol Groups Test of determinative induction. Part Two: Psychometric Properties and interim standarization of test. CSIR Special Report PERS 196. Johannesburg: National Institute for Personnel Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, W., and Broverman, D.M. (1964). Relationship between EEG and test intelligence: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 63, 132–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waber, D.P. (1977). Sex differences in mental abilities, hemispheric lateralization, and rate of physical growth at adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 13, 29–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waber, D.P. (1979). Cognitive abilities and sex-related variations in the maturation of cerebral cortical functions. In H.A. Wittig and A.C. Petersen (Eds.) Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Developmental issues. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallbrown, F.H., Blaha, J., and Wherry, R.J. (1974). The hierarchical factor structure of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 44, 47–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wandel, D., and Royce, J.R. (1978). Toward a multi-factor theory of styles and their relationships to cognition and affect. Journal of Personality, 46, 474–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J.B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviourist views it. Psychological review, 20, 158–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1967). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinberg, H. (1969). Correlation of frequency spectra of averaged visual evoked potentials and verbal intelligence. Nature, 224, 813–815.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, D.J. (1973). The stratified adaptive computerized ability test. AD 768376/0GA. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, D.J. (1974). Strategies of adaptive ability measurement. Research Report 74–5. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota. NTIS No AD A004270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, D.J. (1978). Proceedings of the 1977 Computerized Adaptive Testing Conference. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, D.J. (1979). Proceedings of the Computerized Adaptive Testing Conference (1979) held at Wayzata, Minnesota. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, G.S., and Baucom, D.H. (1977). Sex, masculinity-femininity, and intelligence. Intelligence, 1, 218–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werdelin, I. (1959). Geometrical ability and the space factors. Lund, Sweden: University of Lund.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wernicke, C. (1874). Der aphasisiche Symptomenkomplex. Breslau: Cohn and Weigert.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werts, C.E. (1967). Paternal influence on career choice. National Merit Scholarship Corporation Research Reports, 3, 19.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, P.O. (1973) Individual differences in speed, accuracy, and persistence: A mathematical model for problem solving. In H.J. Eysenck (Ed.) The measurement of intelligence. London: Tinling.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiteley, S., and Schneider, L.M. (1981). Information structure for geometric analogies: A test theory approach. Applied Psychological Measurement, 5, 383–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wickelgren, W.A. (1977). Speed-accuracy trade-off and information processing dynamics. Acta Psychologia, 41, 67–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winograd, T. (1980). What does it mean to understand language? Cognitive Science, 4, 209–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wissler, C. (1901). The correlation of mental and physical tests. Psychological Review Monograph Supplement, 3, (6).

    Google Scholar 

  • Witkin, H.A., and Berry, J.W. (1975). Psychological differentiation in cross-cultural perspective. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 6, 4–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittig, M.A. (1979). Genetic influences on sex-related differences in intellectual performance: Theoretical and methodological issues. In M.A. Wittig and A.C. Petersen (Eds.) Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Developmental issues. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittig, M.A., and Petersen, A.C. (1979). (Eds.) Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning: Developmental issues. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D.A. (1962). Louis Leon Thurstone: Creative thinker, dedicated teacher, eminent psychologist. Princeton: New Jersey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodruff, D.S. (1978). Brain electrical activity and behaviour relationships over the life-span. In P.B. Baltes (Eds.) Life-span Development and Behaviour, Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1862). On the methods of psychology. Translated from Beitrage zur Theorie der Sinneswahrnegnung. Leipzig: C.F. Winton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yen, W.M. (1975). Sex-linked major gene influences on selected types of spatial performance. Behaviour Genetics, 5, 281–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yule, G.U. (1897). On the significance of Bravais Formulae for regression, etc., in skew correlation. Proceedings of the Royal Society, 60, 477–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zangwill, O.L. (1974). Consciousness and the cerebral hemispheres. In S.L. Dimond and J.G. Beaumont (Eds.) Hemisphere function in the human brain. London: Elek. Science.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1987 Martinus Nijhoff Publishers, Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Verster, J.M. (1987). Human Cognition and Intelligence Towards an Integrated Theoretical Perspective. In: Irvine, S.H., Newstead, S.E. (eds) Intelligence and Cognition: Contemporary Frames of Reference. NATO ASI Series, vol 38. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-010-9437-5_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-010-9437-5_2

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-90-247-3523-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-010-9437-5

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics