Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Studies in Cognitive Systems ((COGS,volume 26))

  • 592 Accesses

Abstract

The current influence of the “behavior-based” approach to robotics (Brooks 1991), as well as the notion of “prerational intelligence” introduced in the present volume, challenge psychologists in one of their traditional fields of inquiry.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Arbib, M.A. (1972). The metaphoric brain. An introduction to cybernetics as artificial intelligence and brain theory. New York: Wiley-Interscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B.J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A.D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, N.A. (1947). O postrojenii dvizhenij [On construction of movements]. Moscow: Medigiz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton, D. (1983, July). Creole languages. Scientific American 249, 108–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton, D. (1984). The language to consciousness. In E.R. John (ed.), Machinery of the mind (pp. 128–156). Cambridge, MA: Birkhäuser.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bidell, T. (1988). Vygotsky, Piaget, and the dialectic of development. Human Development 31, 329–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Binet, A., & T. Simon (1916). The development of intelligence in children. Trans. by Elizabeth S. Kite. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brentano, F.C. (1874). Psychologie vom empirischen Standpunkt. Leipzig: Duncker und Humblot.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bridgeman, B. (1986). Relations between the physiology of attention and the physiology of consciousness. Psychological Research 48, 259–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bridgeman, B. (1988). The biology of behavior and mind. Ch. 14: Consciousness and high-level control. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bridgeman, B. (1992a). Conscious versus unconscious processes: The case of vision. Theory and Psychology (Sage) 2(1), 73–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bridgeman, B. (1992b) The evolution of consciousness and language. Psycolo-quy. 92. Consciousness. 1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, R.A.(1991). New approaches to robotics. Science 253, 1227–1232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan, J. (1812). The philosophy of human nature. Richmond, Kentucky: Grimes.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cellérier, G. (1992). Le constructivisme génétique d’aujourd’hui. In B. Inhelder & G. Cellérier (eds.), Le cheminement des découvertes de l’enfant (pp. 217–253). Neuchâtel, Switzerland, and Paris: Delachaux et Niestlé.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, L.A., & D.L. Schacter (1992). Dissociations between structural and episodic representations of visual objects. Current Directions in Psychological Science 1, 141–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F., & R. Lockhart (1972). Levels of processing; A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 11, 671–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dean, A.L. (1994). Instinctual affective forces in the internalization process: Contribution of Hans Loewald. Human Development 37, 42–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ebbinghaus, H. (1985). Über das Gedächtnis. Leipzig, Germany: Duncker & Hum-blot.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G.M. (1987). Neural Darwinism. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, G. (1985). Supplementary motor area structure and function: Review and hyptheses. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8, 567–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Head, H. (1920). Studies in neurology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1985). Wünschen — Wählen — Wollen. In H. Heckhausen, RM. Gollwitzer, & RE. Weinert (eds.), Jenseits des Rubikon: Der Wille in den Humanwissenschaften (pp. 3–9). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1991). Motivation and action. Berlin: Springer Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hirst, W. (1993). On the nature of systems. In G. Harman (ed.), Conceptions of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder, B., & G. Cellérier (1992). Le cheminement des découvertes de l’enfant. Lausanne, Switzerland: Delachaux et Niestle.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890 [1950]). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod, M. (1994).The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17, 187–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimble, G.A., & L.C. Perlmuter (1970). The problem of volition. Psychological Reviews 77, 361–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koehler, W. (1925). The mentality of apes. New York: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolb, B., & I.Q. Wishaw (1990). Fundamentals of human neuropsychology. San Francisco, CA: W.H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolers, P. (1975). Specificity of operations in sentence recognition. Cognitive Psychology 7, 289–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A.R. (1981). Language and cognition. Ed. and translated by J.V. Wertsch. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • McFarland, D. (1989). The teleological imperative. In A. Montefiore & D. Noble (eds.), Goals, no-goals and own-goals (pp. 211–228). London: Unwin Hyman.

    Google Scholar 

  • McFarland, D., & T. Bösser (1993). Intelligent behavior in animals and robots. A Bradford Book. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G.A. (1956). The magical number seven plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review 63, 81–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G.A., E.H. Galanter, & K.H. Pribram (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt Rinehart & Winston.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Minsky, M.L. (1986). The society of mind. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & H.A. Simon (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D.A., & T. Shallice (1980). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. Technical Report 8006, Center for Human Information Processing. San Diego, CA: University of California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakley, D.A. (1985). Brain and mind. New York: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paillard, J. (1982). Apraxia and the neurophysiology of motor control. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B298, 111–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paillard, J. (1986). Development and acquisition of motor skills: A challenging prospect for neuroscience. In H.T.A. Whitting & M.G. Wade (eds.), Motor development in children: Aspects of coordination and control (pp. 416–441). Den Haag, The Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paillard, J. (1987). Cognitive versus sensorimotor encoding of spatial information. In P. Ellen & C. Thinus-Blanc (eds.), Cognitive processes and spatial orientation in animal and man (pp. 43–77). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Paillard, J. (1994). L’intégration sensori-motrice et idéo-motrice. In M. Richelle, J. Requin, & M. Robert (eds.), Traité de psychologie expérimentale (pp. 925–961). Paris: Presses Univérsitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paillard, J. (1999). Motor determinants of a unified word perception. In G. Aschersleben, T. Bachmann, & J. Müsseier (eds.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (pp. 95–111). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Paillard, J. (2000). The neurobiological roots of rational thinking. In H. Cruse, H. Ritter, & J. Dean (eds.), Prerational intelligence: Adaptive behavior and intelligent systems without symbols and logic, Vol. 1 (pp. 343–355). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parkin, A.J., & S. Streete (1988). Implicit and explicit memory in young children and adults. British Journal of Psychology 79 (3), 361–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952). The origin of intelligence in children. New York: Norton.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1983). Piaget’s theory. In RH. Mussen (ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol1. New York: Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z.W. (1981). The imagery debate: Analogue media versus tacit knowledge. Psychological Review 88, 16–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. (1971). Mind in evolution. Boston, MA: Houghton-Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roland, RE., B. Larsen, N.A. Larsen, & E. Skinhoj (1980). Supplementary motor area and other cortical areas in the organization of voluntary movements in man. Journal of Neurophysiology 43, 118–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. New York: Barnes and Noble.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D.L., & E. Tulving (1954). Memory systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T. (1978). The dominant action system: An informal model. In K. Pope & J.E. Singer (eds.), The stream of consciousness: Scientific investigation into the flow of conscious experience (pp. 117–157). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimamura, A.P. (1994). The neuropsychology of metacognition. In J.J. Metacalfe & A.P. Shimamura (eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Standing, L. (1973). Learning 10,000 pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 25, 207–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, J. (1994). The implications for understanding high-level cognition of a grounding in elementary adaptive systems. In P. Gausser & J.-D. Nicoud (eds.), From perception to action (pp. 312–317). Los Alamitos, CA: IEEE Computer Society Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, G.J. (1984). Memory: Time binding in organisms. In L.R. Squire & N. Butters (eds.), Neuropsychology of memory (pp. 374–384). London: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E.L. (1913). The psychology of learning. New York: Teachers College.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., C.A. Hayman, & C. MacDonald (1991). Long-lasting perceptual priming and semantic learning in amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 17, 595–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velichkovsky, B.M. (1982). Visual cognition and its spatial-temporal context. In F. Klix, J. Hoffmann, & E. van der Meer (eds.), Cognitive research in psychology (pp. 47–58). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Velichkovsky, B.M. (1990). The vertical dimension of mental functioning. Psychological Research 52, 282–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velichkovsky, B.M. (1994). The levels endeavour in psychology and cognitive science. In P. Bertelson, P. Eelen, & G. d’Ydewalle (eds.), International perspectives on psychological science: Leading themes (pp. 143–158). Hove, UK, and Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Velichkovsky, B.M. (1999). From levels of processing to stratification of cognition. In B.H. Challis & B.M. Velichkovsky (eds.), Stratification in cognition and consciousness (pp. 203–226). Amsterdam: John Bejamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L.S. (1962). Thought and language. Ed. and translated by E. Hanfmann and G. Vakar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind and society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J.V (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2000 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Bridgeman, B., Cellerier, G., Paillard, J., Velichkovsky, B. (2000). Prerational Intelligence from the Perspective of Psychology. In: Cruse, H., Dean, J., Ritter, H. (eds) Prerational Intelligence: Adaptive Behavior and Intelligent Systems Without Symbols and Logic, Volume 1, Volume 2 Prerational Intelligence: Interdisciplinary Perspectives on the Behavior of Natural and Artificial Systems, Volume 3. Studies in Cognitive Systems, vol 26. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-010-0870-9_80

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-010-0870-9_80

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-94-010-3792-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-010-0870-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics