Beck, J. (1975). The perception of surface color. Scientific American, 233 (2), 62–75.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Bregman, A.S. (1977). Perception and behavior as compositions of ideals. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 250–292.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Bregman, A.S. (1978). Auditory streaming: competition among alternative organizations. Perception and Psychophysics, 23, 391–398.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Bregman, A.S. and Rudnicky, A. (1975). Auditory segregation: stream or streams? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1. 263–267.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Broadbent, D.E. and Ladefoged, P. (1957). On the fusion of sounds reaching different sense organs. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 29, 708–710.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Collins, S. (1985). Duplex perception with musical stimuli: A further investigation. Perception and Psychophysics, 38, 172–177.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Crowder, R.G. (1982). A common basis for auditory sensory storage in perception and immediate memory. Perception and Psychophysics, 31, 477–483.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Cutting, J.E. (1976). Auditory and linguistic processes in speech perception: Inferences from six fusions in dichotic listening. Psychological Review, 83, 114–140.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Darwin, C.J. (1981). Perceptual grouping of speech components differing in fundamental frequency and onset-time. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 185–207.
Google Scholar
Fitch, H.L., Halwes, T., Erickson, D.M., and Liberman, A.M. (1980). Perceptual equivalence of two acoustic cues for stop-consonant manner. Perception and Psychophysics, 27, 343–350.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Hafter, E.R. and Jeffress, L.A. (1968). Two-image lateralization of tones and clicks. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 44, 563–569.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Kanizsa, G. (1955). Margini quasi-percettivi in campi con stimulazione omogenea. Revista di Psicologia, 49, 7–30.
Google Scholar
Liberman, A.M. (1982). On finding that speech is special. American Psychologist, 37, 148–167.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Mann, V.A., Madden, J., Russell, J.M., and Liberman, A.M. (1981). Further investigations into the influence of preceding liquids on stop consonant perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 69, S91. (Abstract).
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Nusbaum, H.C., Schwab, E.C., and Sawusch, J.R. (1983). The role of “chirp” identification in duplex perception. Perception and Psychophysics, 33, 323–332.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Pastore, R.E., Schmuckler, M.A., Rosenbaum, L., and Szczesiul, R. (1983). Duplex perception with musical stimuli. Perception and Psychophysics, 33, 469–474.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Pisoni, D.B. (1973). Auditory and phonetic codes in the discrimination of consonants and vowels. Perception and Psychophysics, 13, 253–260.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Rand, T.C. (1974). Dichotic release from masking for speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 55, 678–680(L).
Google Scholar
Repp, B.H. (1984). Against a role of “chirp” identification in duplex perception. Perception and Psychophysics, 35, 89–93.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Siegel, W. and Siegel, J.A. (1976). Categorical perception of tonal intervals: Musicians can’t tell sharp from flat. Research Bulletin No. 366, Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario.
Google Scholar
Steiger, H. (1983). Influences of sequential organization processes on the use of binaural cues. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, McGill University.
Google Scholar