Advertisement

Cortical Maturation and Perceptual Development

  • Mark H. Johnson
Part of the NATO ASI Series book series (ASID, volume 56)

Abstract

The question of how the postnatal maturation of the brain influences perceptual, motor and cognitive development is a difficult one. In this chapter I begin by focussing on how the postnatal growth of cortical sensory pathways may influence the development of vision, perception and attention. Next, I consider some ways that a cortical system may gain control over behavior, before concluding with some speculative remarks on the developmental processes which may result in particular cortical pathways becoming specialised for processing certain classes of input in the adult. While earlier sections concentrate on how perceptual development can be accounted for by the maturation of neural pathways, the last section outlines a specific role for experiential factors in brain development.

Keywords

Face Recognition Superior Colliculus Middle Temporal Face Pattern Cortical Pathway 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abramov, I., Gordon, J., Hendrickson, A., Hainline, L., Dobson, V. & Laboussier, E. (1982). The retina of the newborn human infant. Science, 217, 265–267.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson, D.A. & Zipser, D. (1988). The role of the posterior parietal cortex in coordinate transformations for visual-motor integration. Canadian Journal of Physiology & Pharmacology, 66, 488–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Aslin, R.N. (1981). Development of smooth pursuit in human infants. In: D.F. Fisher, R.A. Monty & J.W. Senders (Eds.). Eye Movements: Cognition and Visual Perception. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, 31–51.Google Scholar
  4. Atkinson, J. (1984). Human visual development over the first six months of life: a review and a hypothesis. Human Neurobiology, 3, 61–74.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Atkinson, J., Hood, B., Wattam-Bell, J., Anker, S. & Tricklebank, J. (1988). Development of orientation discrimination in infants. Perception, 17, 587–595.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bower, T.G.R. (1974). Development in Infancy. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
  7. Bronson, G.W. (1974). The postnatal growth of visual capacity. Child Development, 45, 873–890.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bronson, G.W. (1982). Structure, status and characteristicsof the nervous system at birth. In: P. Stratton (Ed.). Psychobiology of the Human Newborn. Chichester: John Wiley Sons.Google Scholar
  9. Bushnell, I.W.R., Sai, F. & Mullin, J.T. (1989). Neonatal recognition of the mother’s face. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 3–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Clarkson, M.G. & Clifton, R.K. (in press) Acoustic determinants of newborn orienting. In: M.J.Weiss & P.R. Zelazo (Eds). Newborn Attention. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex.Google Scholar
  11. Clifton, R.K., Morrongiello, B.A., Kulig, J.W., & Dowd, J.M. (1981). Newborns’ orientation toward sound: possible implications for cortical development. Child Development, 52, 833–838.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Conel, J.L. (1939–1967). The Postnatal Development of the Human Cerebral Cortex, Vols. I–VIII. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Courten, de C., & Garey, L.J. (1982). Morphology of the neurones in the human lateral geniculate nucleus and their normal development. Exp. Brain Res., 47, 159–171.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. de Schonen, S. & Mathivet, H. (1989). First come, first served: a scenario about the development of hemispheric specialisation in face recognition during infancy. European Bulletin of cognitive Psychology, 9, 3–44..Google Scholar
  15. DeYoe, E.A. & Van Essen, D.C. (1989). Concurrent processing streams in monkey visual cortex. TINS, 11, 219–226.Google Scholar
  16. Diamond, A. (1988). Differences between adult and infant cognition: is the crucial variable presence or absence of language? In: L. Weiskrantz (Ed.) Thought without Language. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  17. Diamond, A. & Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1983). Comparison of performance on a Piagetian object permanence task in human infants and rhesus monkeys: evidence for involvement of prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience Abstracts, 9, 641.Google Scholar
  18. Diamond, A. & Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1985). Evidence for the involvement of the prefrontal cortex in cognitive changes during the first year of life. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 11, 832.Google Scholar
  19. Goren, C.C., Sarty, M. & Wu, P.Y.K. (1975). Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. Pediatrics, 56, 544–549.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Gottlieb, G. (1980). Development of species identification in ducklings VI: Specific embryonic experience required to maintain species-typical perception in Peking ducklings. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 94, 579–587.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Granger, R., Ambrose-Ingerson, J. & Lynch, G. (1989). Derivation of encoding characteristics of layer II cerebral cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 61–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Haith, M.M., Hazan, C. & Goodman, G.S. (1988). Expectation and anticipation of dynamic visual events by 35-month old babies. Child Development, 59, 467–479.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Johnson, M.H. (1988). Memories of mother. New Scientist, 1600, 60–62.Google Scholar
  24. Johnson, M.H. (1989). Cortical maturation and the development of visual attention in early infancy. London: MRC Cognitive Development Unit.Google Scholar
  25. Johnson, M.H. (in press) Information processing and storage during filial imprinting. In: P.G. Hepper (Ed.). Kin Recognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University press.Google Scholar
  26. Johnson, M.H. & Morton, J. (in press). The Development of Face Recognition, Oxford: Blackwells.Google Scholar
  27. Johnson, M.H., Dziurawiec, S., Bartrip, J. & Morton, J. (subnitted for publication). Infants’ preferences for face-like stimuli: effects of the movement of internal features.Google Scholar
  28. Johnson, M.H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H.D. & Morton, J. (Submitted for publication). Newborns’ preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline.Google Scholar
  29. Kolb, B. (1989). Brain development, plasticity, and behavior. American Psychologist, 44, 1203–1212.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Leuba, G. & Garey, L.J. (1982). A morphometric developmental study of dendrites in the lateral geniculate of the monkey. Neurosci. 7, (suppl) 131.Google Scholar
  31. Maratos. O. (1982). Trends in the development of early imitation in infancy. In: T.G. Bever (Ed.). Regressions in development: basic phenomena and theories. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  32. Maunsell, J.H.R. & Van Essen, D.C. (1983). The connections of the middle temporal visual area (MT) and their relation to a cortical hierarchy in the macaque monkey. Journal of Neuroscience, 3, 2563–2586.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Maurer, D. & Barrera, M. (1981). Infants’ perception of natural and distorted arrangements of a schematic face. Child Development, 47, 523–527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Maurer, D. & Lewis, T.L. (1979). A physiological explanation of infants’ early visual development. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 33, 232–252.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Maurer, D. & Young, R.E. (1983). Newborns’ following of natural and distorted arrangements of facial features. Infant Behavior & Development, 6, 127–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. McClelland, J. & Rumelhart, D. (1986). Parallel Distributed Processing. Vol. 2. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  37. McGraw, M.B. (1943). The neuromuscular maturation of the human infant. New York: Columbia University press.Google Scholar
  38. Morton, J. & Johnson, M.H. (Submitted for publication). Conspec and Conlern: A two-process theory of infant face recognition.Google Scholar
  39. Morton, J., Johnson, M.H. & Maurer, D. (in press) On the reasons for newborns responses to faces. Infant Behavior & Development,Google Scholar
  40. Muir, D.W., Clifton, R.K. & Clarkson, M.G. (1989). The development of a human auditory localization response: a U-shaped function. Canadian Journal of psychology, 43, 199–216.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Perrett, D.I., Rolls, E.T. & Caan, W, (1982). Visual neurones responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex. Exp. Brain Res., 47, 229–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Posner, M.I. & Rothbart, M.K. (1980). The development of attentional mechanisms. In: Flower, J.H. (Ed.). Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln, Nebraska: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  43. Posner, M.I. & Rothbart, M.K. (1989). Attention: normal and pathological development. University of Oregon Institute of Cognitive & Decision Sciences Report No. 89-11.Google Scholar
  44. Rakic, P. (1976). Prenatal genesis of connections subserving ocular dominance in the rhesus monkey. Nature, 261, 461–471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rakic, P. (1983). Geniculo-cortical connections in primates: normal and experimentally altered development. Prog. Brain Research, 58, 393–404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Robinson, N.S., McCarty, M.E. & Haith, M.M. (1988). Visual expectations in early infancy. Paper presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, Washington: D.C.Google Scholar
  47. Rockland, K.S. & Pandya, D.N. (1979). Laminar origins and terminations of cortical connections of the occipital lobe in the rhesus monkey. Brain Research, 179, 3–20.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Schiller, P.H. (1985). A model for the generation of visually guided saccadic eye movements. In: D. Rose & V.G. Dobson (Eds.). Models of the Visual Cortex. Chicester: John Wiley Sons.Google Scholar
  49. Slater, A., Morison, V. & Somers, M. (1988). Orientation discrimination and cortical function in the human newborn. Perception, 17, 597–602.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Stampalua, A. & Kostovic, I. (1981). The laminar organization of the superior colliculus (SC) in the human fetus. In: A. Huber & D. Klein (Eds.) Neurogenetics and Neuro-ophthalmology. North Holland: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  51. Vinter, A. (1986). The role of movement in eliciting early imitations. Child Development, 57, 66–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Van Essen, D.C. (1985). functional organisation of primate visual cortex. In: A. Peters & E.G. Jones (Eds.,). Cerebral Cortex, Vol.3,. Plenum Publishing corp.Google Scholar
  53. Von Hofsten, C. (1984). Developmental changes in the organisation of prereaching movements. Developmental Psychology, 20, 378–388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Yakovlev, P.I. & Lecours, A. (1967). The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain. In: A. Minokowski (Ed.). Regional Development of the Brain in Early Life. Philadelphia: Davis.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mark H. Johnson

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations