Skip to main content

James Mark Baldwin, the Baldwin Effect, Organic Selection, and the American “Immigrant Crisis” at the Turn of the Twentieth Century

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Entangled Life

Part of the book series: History, Philosophy and Theory of the Life Sciences ((HPTL,volume 4))

Abstract

The “Baldwin Effect,” named after the turn-of-the-twentieth-century American psychologist James Mark Baldwin, has experienced a revival over the last few decades, driven primarily by some cognitive scientists who think it might be able to solve problems related to the evolution of consciousness. Baldwin’s own interests when he developed the theory, which he called “organic selection,” were somewhat different from those of modern cognitivists, and his social context was enormously different. This chapter aims to recover the social challenges of Baldwin’s time and explore how they might have been related to his proposal. Chief among these challenges was the widespread perception in the United States that the massive immigrant slums in New York and other cities posed a kind of existential threat to the American way of life. This perception, in turn, led to a number of radical and disturbing eugenic proposals for meeting the “immigrant problem.” It is suggested here that, although Baldwin did not address the immigrant issue directly, it was in his mind as he developed his theory of “organic selection,” and also that it offered a way out of the crisis that many Americans thought they then faced.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    For a recent account of Osborn’s psychological research while at Princeton, see Young (2012) (and for more context, Young 2009).

  2. 2.

    Exact figures vary somewhat from source to source. Two particularly accessible and reliable sources are the Ellis Island Foundation’s own timeline (http://bit.ly/3fYIo6) and the Fordham University website on New York City History (http://bit.ly/zJpFnb).

  3. 3.

    See Camille Avena’s essay “Progressive education in New York City” on the Fordham University website on New York City history (http://bit.ly/oyGTs2).

  4. 4.

    Cited in Ric Burns’ (1999) “New York: A Documentary Film,” Episode 4, 34:00 http://www.pbs.org/wnet/newyork/.

  5. 5.

    Jordan himself did not take this to be a “just criticism,” however, because, essentially, whatever is true is true, whether it lead to despair or no. He also noted that Osborn was predicting the rapid decline of Weismann’s influence in 1892. Of course, it was Osborn’s Lamarckism that rapidly declined, and Osborn himself became a eugenicist before long.

  6. 6.

    It is easy to become distracted by the inclusion of the term “ethical” in the title of the second book, but the matter is explained in Baldwin’s autobiography (Baldwin 1926, 66–67). Essentially he added the term to the title at the last minute in order to make it more appealing to the Danish Royal Academy of Sciences awards committee. Originally the book was subtitled just “A Study in Social Psychology.”

  7. 7.

    See Wozniak (2009) for a detailed account of the professional disaster that befell Baldwin in the wake of his arrest and resignation.

  8. 8.

    There is little reason to believe that Baldwin’s writings had much to do with that decision. Baldwin and Woodrow Wilson had despised each other when, respectively, professor and president at Princeton. Indeed, Wilson’s high-handedness is one of the reasons Baldwin cites for having left Princeton for Hopkins in 1905. It was the “Zimmerman telegram” from Germany, urging Mexico to attack the US in exchange for a return of New Mexico and Arizona after the war that prompted Wilson to act in 1917. Incidentally, a ship on which Baldwin and his family were travelling across the English Channel, the Sussex, was torpedoed and sunk during WWI. One of his daughters was seriously wounded in the attack. A young Wilder Penfield, who would later become a leading neuroscientist in Montréal, was on the same ship, and became acquainted with the Baldwin family there.

  9. 9.

    Though, see Baldwin’s (1902, 144–148) comments on “Social Progress.”

  10. 10.

    Thanks to Robert Wozniak for directing me to this passage. He believes that by ‘superstition,’ Baldwin probably meant Catholicism and, if so, then he probably felt the same way about the Irish and German immigrants of the mid-nineteenth century, and the Italian immigrants of the late nineteenth as well.

  11. 11.

    Dewey (1898) published a review of Baldwin’s Social and Ethical Interpretations, but it did not touch on the present issues either, focusing mainly on what Dewey took to be a certain theoretical confusion in Baldwin’s view between the process of developing a conscious self and the specific contents of that consciousness.

References

  • Baldwin, J. Mark. 1895a. Differences in children, from the teacher’s point of view [Part 1]. The Inland Educator 1: 5–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. Mark. 1895b. Differences in pupils from the teacher’s point of view [Part 2]. The Inland Educator 1: 269–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. Mark. 1895c. Mental development in the child and the race. New York: Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. Mark. 1896a. A new factor in evolution. American Naturalist 30: 441–451. 536–553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. Mark. 1896b. Differences in pupils from the teacher’s point of view [Part 3]. The Inland Educator 2: 126–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. Mark. 1896c. Differences in pupils from the teacher’s point of view [Part 4]. The Inland Educator 3: 232–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. Mark. 1897a. Invention vs. imitation in children [Part 1]. The Inland Educator 4: 267–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. Mark. 1897b. Invention vs. imitation in children [Part 2]. The Inland Educator 5: 58–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. Mark. 1897c. Social and ethical interpretations in mental development. New York: Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. Mark. 1902. Development and evolution. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. Mark. 1909. Darwin and the humanities. Baltimore: Review.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. Mark. 1926. Between two wars, 1861–1921: Being memories, opinions and letters received. Boston: Stratford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cope, E.D. 1887. The origin of the fittest: Essays on evolution. New York: Appleton.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, Charles. 1871. The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex, 1st ed. London: John Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, Terrence W. 1997. The symbolic species. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, Daniel C. 1995. Darwin’s dangerous idea. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, John. 1898. [Review of] Social and ethical interpretations of mental development. Philosophical Review 7: 398–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, P.E. 2003. Beyond the Baldwin effect: James Mark Baldwin’s “social heredity,” epigenetic inheritance, and niche-construction. In Evolution and learning: The Baldwin effect reconsidered, ed. Bruce H. Weber and David J. Depew, 193–215. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinton, Geoffrey E., and Steven J. Nolan. 1987. How learning can guide evolution. Complex Systems 1: 495–502.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huxley, Julian. 1942. Evolution: The modern synthesis. London: Allen and Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • [James, William]. 1875. [Review of] Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie. North American Review 121: 195–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, David Starr. 1892. The present battle-ground of evolution. The Dial 13 (October 16): 242–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, John. 1987. Natural selection: When learning guides evolution. Nature 329: 761–762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCosh, James. 1890. The religious aspect of evolution. New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearce, Trevor. 2010. “A perfect chaos”: Organism-environment interaction and the causal factors of evolution. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riis, Jacob A. 1890. How the other half lives: Studies among the tenements of New York. New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, George J. 1877. The scientific evidences of organic evolution. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, George J. 1882. Animal intelligence. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, George J. 1888. Mental evolution in man. London: Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, George J. 1893a. Mr. Herbert Spencer on “natural selection”. Contemporary Review 63: 499–517.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, George J. 1893b. The Spencer-Weismann controversy. Contemporary Review 64: 50–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, George J. 1893c. A note on panmixia. Contemporary Review 64: 611–612.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, George Gaylord. 1953. The Baldwin effect. Evolution 7: 110–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, Herbert. 1893a. The inadequacy of “natural selection”. Contemporary Review 63: 153–166, 439–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, Herbert. 1893b. Professor Weismann’s theories. Contemporary Review 63: 743–760.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, Herbert. 1893c. Rejoinder to Professor Weismann. Contemporary Review 64: 893–912.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waddington, Conrad. 1942. Canalization of development and the inheritance of acquired characters. Nature 150: 563–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waddington, Conrad. 1953a. Genetic assimilation of an acquired character. Evolution 7: 118–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waddington, Conrad. 1953b. “The Baldwin effect,” “genetic assimilation,” and homeostasis. Evolution 7: 386–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, Bruce H., and David J. Depew (eds.). 2003. Evolution and learning: The Baldwin effect reconsidered. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weismann, August. 1885. Die Kontinuität des Keimplasmas als Grundlage einer Theorie der Vererbung [The continuity of the germ-plasm as the principle of a theory of heredity]. Jena: Gustav Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weismann, August. 1892. Das Keimplasma: Eine Theorie der Vererbung [The germ-plasm: A theory of heredity]. Jena: Gustav Fisher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weismann, August. 1893. The all-sufficiency of natural selection. Contemporary Review 64: 309–338, 596–610.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weismann, August. 1894. The effect of external influences upon development. London: Henry Frowde, Amen Corner. (Originally delivered as the Romanes Lecture for 1894 at Oxford.).

    Google Scholar 

  • Wozniak, Robert H. 2009. James Mark Baldwin, professional disaster, and the European connection. Rassegna di Psicologia 26: 111–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, Chauncey. 1870. Limits of natural selection. North American Review 111 (October): 282–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, Chauncey. 1871. The genesis of species. North American Review 113 (July): 63–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, Chauncey. 1873. Evolution of self-consciousness. North American Review 116 (April): 245–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, Jacy L. 2009. Evolution, education, and eugenics: Organic selection in progressive era America. M.A. thesis, York University, Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, Jacy L. 2012. The biologist as psychologist: Henry Fairfield Osborn’s early mental ability investigations. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 48: 197–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christopher D. Green .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Green, C.D. (2014). James Mark Baldwin, the Baldwin Effect, Organic Selection, and the American “Immigrant Crisis” at the Turn of the Twentieth Century. In: Barker, G., Desjardins, E., Pearce, T. (eds) Entangled Life. History, Philosophy and Theory of the Life Sciences, vol 4. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-7067-6_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics