Advertisement

The Emergence of Empathy in the Context of Cross-Species Mind Reading

Chapter
Part of the Biosemiotics book series (BSEM, volume 8)

Abstract

Evolutionary accounts of the origins of mind reading and empathy have emphasized the reproductive and social value of understanding other human minds. On this view, selective pressures within human communities contributed to our capacity to imagine ourselves in the spatiotemporal and cognitive place of other individuals. I argue that these social accounts of empathy neglect the phenomenon of mind reading between humans and other species. In particular, I argue that the cognitive demands on early human hunters privileged the ability to take on the perspective of potential prey in tracking. These selective pressures on mind reading not only have serious consequences for how we view empathy but may also have had substantive consequences for how we read other human minds.

Keywords

Brain Size Perspective Taking Social Account Hunting Strategy Mind Reading 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Alden, E. (2004). Why do good hunters have higher reproductive success? Human Nature, 15(4), 343–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baron-Cohen, S. (2003). The essential difference: Men, women and the extreme male brain. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). Why so few women in math and science? In C. Sommers (Ed.), The science on women and science (pp. 7–23). Washington, DC: American Enterprise Institute for Public Policy Research.Google Scholar
  4. Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. London/New York: Random House/Basic Books.Google Scholar
  5. Bramble, D., & Lieberman, D. (2004). Endurance running and the evolution of homo-erectus. Nature, 432(7015), 345–352.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Carrier, D. (1984). The energetic paradox of human running and hominid evolution. Current Anthropology, 25(4), 483–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carruthers, P. (1996). Simulation and self-knowledge: A defence of the theory-theory. In P. Carruthers & P. K. Smith (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Carruthers, P. (2002). The roots of scientific reasoning: Infancy, modularity and the art of tracking. In P. Carruthers, S. Stich, & M. Siegal (Eds.), The cognitive basis of science (pp. 73–96). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Currie, G. (2011). Empathy for objects. In A. Coplan & P. Goldie (Eds.), Empathy: Philosophical and psychological perspectives (pp. 82–98). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Curtis, G. (2006). The cave painters: Probing the mysteries of the world’s first artists. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
  11. Dunbar, R. (1992). Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 22(6), 469–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dunbar, R. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(4), 681–735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dunbar, R. (2000). The origin of the human mind. In P. Carruthers & A. Chamberlain (Eds.), Evolution and the human mind (pp. 238–253). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Goldman, A. (2006). Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology and neuroscience of mindreading. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Gurven, M., & Hill, K. (2009). Why do men hunt? A re-evaluation of “Man the Hunter” and the sexual division of labor. Current Anthropology, 50(1), 51–74.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gurven, M., & von Rueden, C. (2006). Hunting, social status and biological fitness. Biodemography and Social Biology, 53(1), 81–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Guthrie, R. Dale (2004). The Nature of Paleolithic Art. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  18. Harrison, M. C. (2011). How narrative relationships overcome empathic bias: Elizabeth Gaskell’s empathy across difference. Poetics Today, 32(2), 255–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hawkes, K. (1991). Showing off: Tests of an hypothesis about men’s foraging goals. Ethology and Sociobiology, 12, 29–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Holekamp, K. E. (2007). Questioning the social intelligence hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Science., 11, 65–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Holliday, T. (1998). The ecological context of trapping among recent hunter-gatherers: Implications for subsistence in terminal Pleistocene Europe. Current Anthropology, 39, 711–719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hrdy, S. (2009). Mothers and others, the evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Humphrey, N. K. (1976). The social function of intellect. In P. P. G. Bateson & R. A. Hinde (Eds.), Growing points in ethology (pp. 303–317). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Humphrey, N. K. (1998). Cave art, autism, and the evolution of the human mind. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 8(2), 165–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Krantz, G. (1968). Brain size and hunting ability in earliest man. Current Anthropology, 9(5), 450–451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Liebenberg, L. W. (1990). The art of tracking: The origin of science. Cape Town: David Philip.Google Scholar
  27. Liebenberg, L. W. (2006). Persistence hunting by modern hunter gatherers. Current Anthropology, 47(6), 1017–1025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Nichols, S. (forthcoming). Mindreading and the philosophy of mind. In J. Prinz (Ed.), The Oxford handbook on philosophy of psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Preston, S., & De Waal, F. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(1), 1–20.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Sellars, W. (1956). Empiricism and the philosophy of mind. Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, 1, 253–329.Google Scholar
  31. Trout, J. D. (2009). The empathy gap: Building bridges to the good life and the good society. New York: Viking/Penguin.Google Scholar
  32. Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of ToledoToledoUSA

Personalised recommendations