Advertisement

Number Sense in Low-Performing Kindergarten Children: Effects of a Working Memory and an Early Math Training

  • Evelyn H. Kroesbergen
  • Jaccoline E. Van’t Noordende
  • Meijke E. Kolkman
Part of the Literacy Studies book series (LITS, volume 6)

Abstract

Previous research has shown that number sense skills can be improved by playing numerical games and that working memory is related to number sense as well. In this chapter, it is investigated if working memory could be trained and what the additional effect could be of a working memory training on children’s number sense skills. Two studies are reported, in which low-performing kindergarten children received training on either number sense or on both number sense and working memory. The results of these studies show that working memory can be trained and moreover, that counting skills can be improved by training working memory. However, for a direct effect, training number sense seems to be the most effective. More research is needed to further examine these results. This chapter provides directions for early interventions for children at risk for mathematical learning problems.

Keywords

Number Line Work Memory Task Number Sense Board Game Work Memory Training 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Alloway, T. P. (2007). Automated working memory assessment. London: Pearson Assessment. [Translated and reproduced by permission of Pearson Assessment.]Google Scholar
  2. Alloway, T. P. (2009). Working memory, but not IQ, predicts subsequent learning in children with learning difficulties. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 25, 92–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Alloway, T. P., & Alloway, R. G. (2010). Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 20–29.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Ansari, D. (2008). Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 279–291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Aunio, P., Hautamäki, J., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2005). Mathematical thinking intervention programmes for preschool children with normal and low number sense. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 20, 131–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 5–28.Google Scholar
  7. Booth, J. L., & Siegler, R. S. (2006). Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. Developmental Psychology, 41, 189–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bryant, D. P. (2005). Commentary on early identification and intervention for students with mathematics difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 340–345.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19, 273–293.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bull, R., Espy, K. A., & Wiebe, S. A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 205–228.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dehaene, S. (2001). Précis of the number sense. Mind & Language, 16, 16–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dowsett, S. M., & Livesey, D. J. (2000). The development of inhibitory control in preschool children: Effects of “executive skills” training. Developmental Psychobiology, 36, 161–174.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Ebersbach, M., Luwel, K., Frick, A., Onghena, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2008). The relationship between the shape of the mental number line and familiarity with numbers in 5- to 9-year-old children: Evidence for a segmented model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99, 1–17.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fuchs, L. S., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Paulsen, K., Bryant, J. D., & Hamlett, C. L. (2005). The prevention, identification, and cognitive determinants of math difficulty. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 493–513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and computation. Cognition, 44, 43–74.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (2000). Assessment of working memory in six- and seven-year-old children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 377–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., & Hamson, C. O. (1999). Numerical and arithmetical cognition: Patterns of functions and deficits in children at risk for a mathematical disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74, 213–239.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Byrd-Craven, J., Nugent, L., & Numtee, C. (2007). Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in children with mathematical learning disability. Child Development, 78, 1343–1359.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., & Byrd-Craven, J. (2008). Development of number line representations in children with mathematical learning disability. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 277–299.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gebuis, T., Cohen-Kadosh, R., De Haan, E., & Henik, A. (2009). Automatic quantity processing in 5-year olds and adults. Cognitive Processing, 10, 133–142.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gersten, R., & Chard, D. (1999). Early math: Rethinking arithmetic instruction for students with mathematical disabilities. The Journal of Special Education, 33, 18–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gersten, R., Jordan, N. C., & Flojo, J. R. (2005). Early identification and interventions for students with mathematics difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 293–304.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., & Dunning, D. L. (2009). Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Developmental Science, 12, F9–F15.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Oláh, L. N., & Locuniak, M. N. (2006). Number sense growth in kindergarten: A longitudinal investigation of children at risk for mathematics difficulties. Child Development, 77, 153–175.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Locuniak, M. N., & Ramineni, C. (2007). Predicting first-grade math achievement from developmental number sense trajectories. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 22, 36–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kalchman, M., Moss, J., & Case, R. (2001). Psychological models for the development of mathematical understanding: Rational numbers and functions. In S. Carver & D. Klahr (Eds.), Cognition and instruction (pp. 1–38). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  27. Kavkler, M., Tancig, S., & Magajna, L. (2003). Longitudinal study of children with very low mathematical competence in preschool years. Paper presented at EARLI2003, Padova, Italy.Google Scholar
  28. Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlström, K., & Westerberg, H. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD: A randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 77–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kolkman, M. E., Hoijtink, H. H., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2012). The role of executive functions in numerical estimation skills [Manuscript submitted for publication].Google Scholar
  30. Krajewski, K., & Schneider, W. (2009). Exploring the impact of phonological awareness, visual-spatial working memory, and preschool quantity-number competencies on mathematics achievement in elementary school: Findings from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 516–531.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kroesbergen, E. H., Van Luit, J. E. H., Van Lieshout, E. C. D. M., Van Loosbroek, E., & Van de Rijt, B. A. M. (2009). Individual differences in early numeracy: The role of executive functions and subitizing. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 27, 226–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kroesbergen, E. H., Van ’t Noordende, J. E., Kolkman, M. E., & Huiting, R. (2012). Math and working memory in kindergarten children: Domain-specific versus domain-general working memory training [Manuscript submitted for publication].Google Scholar
  33. Laski, E. V., & Siegler, R. S. (2007). Is 27 a big number? Correlational and causal connections among numerical categorization, number line estimation, and numerical magnitude comparison. Child Development, 78, 1723–1743.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Locuniak, M. N., & Jordan, N. C. (2008). Using kindergarten number sense to predict calculation fluency in second grade. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 451–459.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Passolunghi, M. C., & Pazzaglia, F. (2005). A comparison of working memory processes in children good or poor in arithmetic word problem-solving. Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 257–269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Passolunghi, M. C., & Siegel, L. S. (2003). Working memory and access to numerical information in children with disability in mathematics. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 348–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Passolunghi, M. C., Mammarella, I. C., & Altoè, G. (2008). Cognitive abilities as precursors of the early acquisition of mathematical skills during first through second grades. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 229–250.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Ramani, G. B., & Siegler, R. S. (2008). Promoting broad and stable improvements in low-income children’s numerical knowledge through playing number board games. Child Development, 79, 375–394.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Rasmussen, C., & Bisanz, J. (2005). Representation and working memory in early arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91, 137–157.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Siegler, R. S., & Ramani, G. B. (2008). Playing linear numerical board games promotes low-income children’s numerical development. Developmental Science, 11, 655–661.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Swanson, H. L., & Beebe-Frankenberger, M. (2004). The relationship between working memory and mathematical problem solving in children at risk and not at risk for serious math difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology, 3, 471–491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Thorell, L. B., Lindqvist, S., Nutley, S. B., Bohlin, G., & Klingberg, T. (2009). Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Developmental Science, 12, 106–113.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Tudge, J. R. H., & Doucet, F. (2004). Early mathematical experiences: Observing young black and white children’s everyday activities. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 21–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Van de Rijt, B. A. M., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (1998). Effectiveness of the additional early mathematics program for teaching children early mathematics. Instructional Science, 26, 337–358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Van de Rijt, B. A. M., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (1999). Milestones in the development of infant numeracy. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 40, 65–71.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Van der Molen, M. (2009). Working memory in children with mild intellectual disabilities: Abilities and training potential. Doctoral dissertation, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
  47. Van der Sluis, S., De Jong, P. F., & Van der Leij, A. (2007). Executive functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading, and arithmetic. Intelligence, 35, 427–449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Van Luit, J. E. H., & Van de Rijt, B. A. M. (2009). Utrechtse Getalbegrip Toets-Revised [Early numeracy test-revised]. Doetinchem: Graviant.Google Scholar
  49. Von Aster, M. G., & Shalev, R. S. (2007). Number development and developmental dyscalculia. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 49, 868–873.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Whyte, J. C., & Bull, R. (2008). Number games, magnitude representation, and basis number skills in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 44, 588–596.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Wilson, K. M., & Swanson, H. L. (2001). Are mathematics disabilities due to a domain-general or a domain-specific working memory deficit? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 237–248.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Witt, M. (2007, August 27–30). Can working memory training affect mathematical performance in children at primary school? Poster presented at Earli 2007, Budapest, Hungary.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Evelyn H. Kroesbergen
    • 1
  • Jaccoline E. Van’t Noordende
    • 1
  • Meijke E. Kolkman
    • 1
  1. 1.Langeveld Institute for the Study of Education and Development in Childhood and AdolescenceUtrecht UniversityUtrechtThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations