Setting Our Own Terms: How We Used Ritual to Become Human

Chapter
Part of the Studies in Neuroscience, Consciousness and Spirituality book series (SNCS, volume 1)

Abstract

Archeological evidence of the sophisticated cognitive attributes thought to define humanity – such as symbolism, language, theory of mind, and a spiritual sense – is, by and large, late-emerging (after 50,000 years before present [ybp]), postdating the emergence of anatomically modern humans (AMH). This suggests that the relevant selection pressures for these abilities did not emerge until after the arrival of the fully human body and brain. I argue that this stands to reason to reason since the selection pressure responsible for the emergence of uniquely human cognition was human-made. Human culture created human cognition. The key facet of that culture was ritual. Ritual selection pressure filtered Homo sapiens sapiens for the very cognitive attributes that made us what we are today.

Keywords

Anterior Cingulate Cortex Work Memory Capacity Meditative Practice Altered State Traditional Society 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Adler, D.S., Bar-Oz, G., Belfer-Cohen, A., & Bar-Yosef, O. (2006). Ahead of the game: Middle and Upper Paleolithic hunting behaviors in the southern Caucasus. Current Anthropology, 47, 89–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Alcorta, C.S. (2006). Religion and the life course: Is adolescence an “experience expectant” period for religious transmission? In P. McNamara (Ed.), Where God and science meet (Vol. 2, pp. 55–79). Bridgeport: Praeger Publishing.Google Scholar
  3. Alley, R.B. (2000). The two-mile time machine: Ice cores, abrupt climate change and our future. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Ambrose, S.H. (1998a). Late Pleistocene population bottlenecks, volcanic winter, and the differentiation of modern humans. Journal of Human Evolution, 34, 623–651.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Ambrose, S.H. (1998b). Chronology of the later stone age and food production in East Africa. Journal of Archaeological Science, 25, 377–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Ambrose, S.H. (2002). Small things remembered: Origins of early microlithic industries in Subsaharan Africa. In R. Elston & S. Kuhn (Eds.), Thinking small: Global perspectives on microlithic technologies (pp. 9–29). Arlington: American Anthropological Association. Archaeological papers of the American Anthropological Association #12, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
  7. Ambrose, S.H., & Lorenz, C.G. (1990). Social and ecological models for the Middle Stone Age in Southern Africa. In P. Mellars (Ed.), The emergence of modern humans (pp. 3–33). Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh Press.Google Scholar
  8. Azari, N.P., Nickel, J.P., Wunderlich, G., Niedeggen, M., Hefter, H., Tellmann, L., et al. (2001). Neural correlates of religious experience. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1649–1652.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Balter, M. (2000). Paintings in Italian cave may be oldest yet. Science, 290, 419–421.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bar-Yosef, O. (2000). A Mediterranean perspective on the Middle/Upper Paleolithic Revolution. In C.B. Stringer, R.N.E. Barton, & J.C. Finlayson (Eds.), Neanderthals on the edge (pp. 9–18). Oxford: Oxbow.Google Scholar
  11. Beauregard, M., & Paquette, V. (2006). Neural correlates of mystical experience in Carmelite nuns. Neuroscience Letters, 405, 186–190.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Beauregard, M., Levesque, J., & Bourgouin, P. (2001). Neurocorrelates of conscious self-regulation of emotion. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(RC 165), 1–6.Google Scholar
  13. Behar, D., Villems, M.R., Soodyall, H., Blue-Smith, J., Pereira, L., et al. (2008). The dawn of human matrilineal diversity. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 82, 1130–1140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bell, C. (1997). Ritual: Perspectives and dimensions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Belyaev, D.K. (1979). Destabilizing selection as a factor in domestication. Journal of Heredity, 70, 301–308.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Bjorklund, D.F., & Rosenberg, J. S. (2005). The role of developmental plasticity in the evolution of human cognition. In B. J. Ellis & D. F. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social mind (pp. 45–75). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  17. Burger, J., Kirchner, M., Bramanti, B., Haak, W., & Thomas, M.G. (2007). Absence of the lactase-persistence-associated allele in early Neolithic Europeans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 3736–3741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Carter, O.L., Presti, D.E., Callistemon, C., Ungerer, Y., Lui, G.B., & Pettigrew, J.D. (2005). Meditation alters perceptual rivalry in Buddhist monks. Current Biology, 15, R412.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Catlin, G. (1867). O-kee-pa: A religious ceremony and other customs of the Mandans. London: Trubner.Google Scholar
  20. Chagnon, N. (1968). Yanamamo: The fierce people. New York: Holt, Rinehart, Winston.Google Scholar
  21. Cunningham, W.A., Johnson, M.K., Raye, C.L., Gatenby, J.C., Gore, J.C., & Banaji, M. (2004). Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces. Psychological Science, 15, 806–813.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Curtis, C.E., & D’Esposito, M. (2003). Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7, 415–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. de Waal, F.B.M. (1990). Peacemaking among primates. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Eliade, M. (1972). Shamanism: Archaic techniques of ecstasy. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  25. Feblot-Augustins, J. (1999). Raw material transport patterns and settlement systems in the European Lower and Middle Paleolithic: Continuity, change, and variability. In W. Roebroeks & C. Gamble (Eds.), The Middle Paleolithic occupation of Europe (pp. 193–214). Leiden: University of Leiden Press.Google Scholar
  26. Frecska, E., & Kulscar, Z. (1989). Social bonding in the modulation of the physiology of ritual dance. Ethos, 17, 70–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Gamble, C. (1999). The Upper Paleolithic societies of Europe. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Glucklich, A. (2001). Sacred pain. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  30. Guenther, M. (1999). From totemism to shamanism: Hunter–gatherer contributions to world mythology and spirituality. In R.B. Lee & R. Daly (Eds.), Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers (pp. 426–433). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Guthrie, R.D. (2005). The nature of Paleolithic art. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  32. Hayden, B. (2003). Shamans, sorcerers and saints: A prehistory of religion. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Books.Google Scholar
  33. Henshilwood, C., d’Errico, F., Vanhaeren, M., van Niekerk, K., & Jacobs, Z. (2004). Middle Stone Age shell beads from South Africa. Nature, 304, 404.Google Scholar
  34. Hoffecker, J.F. (2002). Desolate landscapes: Ice-age settlement in Europe. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
  35. Ingvar, D.H. (1994). The will of the brain: Cerebral correlates of willful acts. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 171, 7–12.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M.J. (1995). Epigenetic inheritance and evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  37. Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M.J. (2005). Evolution in four dimensions. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  38. Kane, M.J., & Engle, R.W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 637–671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Katz, R. (1982). Boiling energy: Community healing among the Kalahari !Kung. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Kelly, A.M., Hester, R., Foxe, J.J., Shpaner, M., & Garavan, H. (2006). Flexible cognitive control: Effects of individual differences and brief practice on a complex cognitive task. NeuroImage, 31, 866–886.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Keverne, E.B., Martinez, N.D., & Tuite, B. (1989). Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of moneys influenced by grooming relationships. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 14, 155–161.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kirschner, M.W., & Gerhart, J.C. (2005). The plausibility of life. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  43. Klein, R.G., & Edgar, B. (2002). The dawn of human culture. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  44. Knight, C.D., Power, C., & Watts, I. (1995). The human symbolic revolution: A Darwinian account. Cambridge Archeological Journal, 5, 75–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Knoch, D., & Fehr, E. (2007). Resisting the power of temptations: The right prefrontal cortex and self-control. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1104, 123–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Lazar, S.W., Bush, G., Gollub, R.L., Fricchione, G.L., Khalsa, G., & Benson, H. (2000). Functional brain mapping of the relaxation response and meditation. NeuroReport, 11, 1581–1585.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Lazar, S.W., Kerr, C.E., Wasserman, R.W., Gray, J.R., Greve, D.N., Treadway, M.T., Mcgarvey, M., Quinn, B.T., Dusek, J.A., Benson, H., Rauch, S.L., Moore, C.I., & Fischl, B. (2005). Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. NeuroReport, 16, 1893–1897.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Lee, R.B., & Daly, R. (1999). Introduction: Foragers and others. In R.B. Lee & R. Daly (Eds.), The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers (pp. 1–22). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  49. Lewis-Williams, D. (2002). The mind in the cave. London: Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
  50. Lou, H.C., Kjaer, T.W., Friberg, L., Wildschiodtz, G., Holm, S., & Nowak, M. (1999). A 15O-H20 PET study of meditation and the resting state of normal consciousness. Human Brain Mapping, 7, 98–105.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Lutkehaus, N.C., & Roscoe, P.B. (1995). Gender rituals: Female initiation in Melanesia. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  52. McClenon, J. (1997). Shamanic healing, human evolution and the origin of religion. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 36, 345–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. McClenon, J. (2002). Wondrous healing: Shamanism, human evolution and the origin of religion. DeKalb: Northern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
  54. Minkel, J.R. (2006). Offerings to a stone snake provide the earliest evidence of religion. Scientific American online http://www.sciam.com/article.cfm?articleID=3FE89A86-E7F2-99DF-366D045A5BF3EAB1. Accessed 21 May 2007.
  55. Mirsky, J. (1937). The Eskimo of Greenland. In M. Mead (Ed.), Cooperation and competition among primitive peoples (pp. 51–86). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  56. Newberg, A.B. (2006). Religious and spiritual practices: A neurochemical perspective. In P. McNamara (Ed.), Where God and science meet (Vol. 2, pp. 15–31). Westport: Praeger.Google Scholar
  57. Newberg, A., Alavi, A., Baime, M., Pourdehnad, M., Santanna, J., & d’Aquili, E. (2001). The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow during the complex cognitive task of meditation: A preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging Section, 106, 113–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Peng, C.K., et al. (2004). Heart rate dynamics during three forms of meditation. International Journal of Cardiology, 95, 19–27.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Petraglia, M., Korisettar, R., Boivin, N., Clarkson, C., Ditchfield, P., et al. (2007). Middle Paleolithic assemblages from the Indian subcontinent before and after the Toba super-eruption. Science, 317, 114–116.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Power, C. (1998). Old wives’ tales: The gossip hypothesis and the reliability of cheap signals. In J.R. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy, & C. Knight (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language: Social and cognitive bases (pp. 111–129). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  61. Rappaport, R.A. (1999). Ritual and religion and the making of humanity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  62. Richerson, P.J., & Boyd, R. (2001). The evolution of subjective commitments to groups: A tribal instincts hypothesis. In R. Neese (Ed.), Evolution and the capacity for commitment (pp. 186–220). New York: Russell Sage.Google Scholar
  63. Richman, B. (1987). Rhythm and melody in gelada vocal exchanges. Primates, 28, 199–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Rossano, M. J. (2010). Supernatural selection: How religion evolved. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Sanfey, A.G., Rilling, J.K., Aronson, J.A., Nystrom, L.E., & Cohen, J.D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science, 300, 1755–1758.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Silk, J.B. (2001). Grunts, girneys, and good intentions: The origins of strategic commitment in nonhuman primates. In R.M. Neese (Ed.), Evolution and the capacity for commitment (pp. 138–158). New York: Russell Sage.Google Scholar
  67. Smuts, B.B., & Watanabe, J.M. (1990). Social relationships and ritualized greetings in adult male baboons (Papio cynocephalus anubis). International Journal of Primatology, 11, 147–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Sorensen, M.V., & Leonard, W.R. (2001). Neandertal energetics and foraging efficiency. Journal of Human Evolution, 40, 483–495.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Sosis, R. (2006). Religious behaviours, badges, and bans: Signally theory and the evolution of religion. In P. McNamara (Ed.), Where God and science meet (Vol. 1, pp. 61–86). Bridgeport: Praeger Publishing.Google Scholar
  70. Stuss, D.T., Binns, M.A., Murphy, K.J., & Alexander, M. P. (2002). Dissociations within the anterior attentional system: Effects of task complexity and irrelevant information on reaction-time speed and accuracy. Neuropsychology, 16, 500–513.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  72. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Townsend, J.B. (1999). Shamanism. In S. D. Glazier (Ed.), Anthropology of religion (pp. 429–469). Westport: Praeger.Google Scholar
  74. Tracey, K.J. (2007). Physiology and immunology of the cholinergic antiinflammatory pathway. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 117, 289–296.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. van Dixhoorn, J., & White, A. (2005). Relaxation therapy for rehabilitation and prevention in ischaemic heart disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. European Journal of Cardiovascular Prevention and Rehabilitation, 12, 193–202.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. van Roosmalen, M.G.M., & Klein, L.L. (1988). The spider monkeys, genus Ateles. In R. Mittermeier, A.B. Rylands, A.F. Coimbra-Filho, & G.A.B. da Fonesca (Eds.), Ecology and behavior of neotropic primates (pp. 445–537). Washington, DC: World Wildlife Fund.Google Scholar
  77. Vanhaeren, M., d’Errico, F., Stringer, C., James, S. L., Todd, J.A., & Mienis, H.K. (2006). Middle Paleolithic shell beads in Israel and Algeria. Science, 312, 1785–1788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Vitebsky, P. (2000). Shamanism. In G. Harvey (Ed.), Indigenous religions (pp. 55–67). London: Cassell.Google Scholar
  79. Waddington, C.H. (1942). Canalization of development and the inheritance of acquired characteristics. Nature, 150, 563–565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Weber, B.H., & Depew, D.J. (2003). Evolution and learning: The Baldwin effect reconsidered. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  81. Whitehouse, H. (1996). Rites of terror: Emotion, metaphor and memory in Melanesian cults. The Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 2, 703–715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Whitham, J.C., & Maestripieri, D. (2003). Primate rituals: The function of greetings between male guinea baboons. Ethology, 109, 847–859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Wiessner, P. (1982). Risk, reciprocity, and social influences on !Kung San economics. In E. Leacock & R. Lee (Eds.), Politics and history in band societies (pp. 61–84). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  84. Winkelman, M. (1990). Shamans and other “magico-religious” healers: A cross-cultural study of their origins, nature, and social transformation. Ethos, 18, 308–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Winkelman, M. (2002). Shamanism and cognitive evolution. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 12, 71–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Wynn, T., & Coolidge, F.L. (2007). Did a small but significant change in working memory capacity empower modern thinking? In P. Mellars, K. Boyle, O. Bar-Yosef, & S. Stringer (Eds.), Rethinking the human evolution: New behavioural and biological perspectives on the origin and dispersal of modern humans (pp. 79–90). Cambridge: Cambridge University McDonald Institute Monographs.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Netherlands 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologySoutheastern Louisiana UniversityHammondUSA

Personalised recommendations