Skip to main content

Controversy as an Event Category

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Controversy as News Discourse

Part of the book series: Argumentation Library ((ARGA,volume 19))

  • 854 Accesses

Abstract

Controversy is a term that journalists (and others) use in their texts and discourse, a feature of language in use. In news discourse it is routinely used as an event category, a way of naming and categorizing the events that journalists report. Journalists narrate controversies using event categories that appear as part of natural phenomenon, historical event, and pragmatic event formulas. They index events through a wide range of selectivity and individuation. The first formula contributes to our experience of controversies as forces of nature, autopoietic processes that develop beyond human agency, decision making, and control. The second and third contribute to our experience of controversies as relatively discrete historical and discursive phenomena, with the third regularly depicting individual human agents as interlocutors. These three formulas not only contribute to narratives of public controversies but also to the shape of the news article as a genre.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

     The terms “foregrounding” and “backgrounding” refer to the degree of salience a particular element of a narrated event will be given depending on the particular kind of utterance used in narrating it. Some posit a kind of “saliency hierarchy” involved in discourse (Fillmore 1977, p. 78). Foregrounded elements will be those that have higher salience in the discourse. Features of high transitivity are associated with high salience, and therefore with foregrounding (Hopper and Thompson 1980, p. 283). These terms can be traced to Prague School linguists (cf. for example Havranek 1932, pp. 9–10; Mukarovsky 1932, p. 29).

References

  • Agha, A. 2007. Language and social relations, Studies in the social and cultural foundations of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aitchison, J. 2007. The word weavers: Newshounds and wordsmiths. Cambridge, UK/New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, A.L. 1995. Beyond translation: Essays toward a modern philology. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, A. 1984. Language style as audience design. Language in Society 13: 145–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, A. 1991. The language of news media. Cambridge: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, J.F. 1988. Events and their names. Indianapolis: Hackett.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benoit, P.J., and D. Hample. 1998. The meaning of two cultural categories: Avoiding interpersonal arguments or cutting them short. In Argument in a time of change: Definitions, frameworks, and critiques, 97–102. Annandale: National Communication Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biber, D. 1988. Variation across speech and writing. Cambridge, UK/New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cramer, P.A. 2008. Controversy as a media event category. In Rhetoric in detail: Discourse analyses of rhetorical talk and text, Discourse approaches to politics, society and culture, vol. 31, 279–305. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dallinger, J.M., and D. Hample. 2002. The image of the ideal arguer. In Arguing communication & culture, vol. 1, 285–291. Washington, DC: National Communication Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dijk, T.A.V. 1988. News as discourse. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Du Bois, J.W. 1980. Beyond definiteness: The trace of identity in discourse. In The pear stories: Cognitive, cultural, and linguistic aspects of narrative production, 203–274. Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fillmore, C.J. 1977. The case for case reopened. In Grammatical relations, Syntax and semantics, vol. 8, 59–81. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, R. 1991. Language in the news: Discourse and ideology in the press. London/New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geisler, C. 1994. Academic literacy and the nature of expertise: Reading, writing, and knowing in academic philosophy. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M.A.K., and R. Hasan. 1976. Cohesion in English. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hample, D., and P.J. Benoit. 1999. Must arguments be explicit and violent: A study of naive social actors’ understandings. In Proceedings of the fourth international conference of the international society for the study of argumentation, 306–310. Amsterdam: SicSat.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hample, D., P.J. Benoit, J. Houston, G. Purifoy, V. VanHyfte, and C. Wardwell. 1999. Naive theories of argument: Avoiding interpersonal arguments or cutting them short. Argumentation and Advocacy 35: 130–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haviland, J.B. 1996. Text from talk in Tzotzil. In Natural histories of discourse, ed. M. Silverstein and G. Urban, 45–78. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Havranek, B. 1932. The functional differentiation of the standard language. In A Prague school reader on esthetics, literary structure, and style, 3–16. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoey, M. 2005. Lexical priming: A new theory of words and language. London/New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, P.J. 1987. Emergent grammar. Berkeley Linguistics Society 13: 139–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, P.J. 1995. The category ‘event’ in natural discourse and logic. In Discourse, grammar, and typology, Studies in language companion series, vol. 27, 139–152. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, P.J. 1998. Emergent grammar. In The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure, 155–175. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, P.J., and S.A. Thompson. 1980. Transitivity in grammar and discourse. Language 56: 251–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, P.J., and S.A. Thompson. 1984. The discourse basis for lexical categories in universal grammar. Language 60: 703–751.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, B. 1994. Repetition in discourse. Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufer, D., S. Ishizaki, B. Butler, and J. Collins. 2004. The power of words: Unveiling the speaker and writer’s hidden craft. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kress, G.R., and B. Hodge. 1979. Language as ideology. London/Boston: Routledge/Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labov, W., and J. Waletzky. 1967. Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience. In Essays on the verbal and visual arts, ed. J. Helm, 12–44. Seattle: American Ethnological Society/University of Washington Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, A.B., S. Mitchell, and G. Nagy. 2000. The singer of tales. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J.R. 1992. English text: System and structure. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mourelatos, A.P. 1981. Events, processes, and states. In Syntax and semantics 14: Tense and aspect, Syntax and semantics, vol. 14, 191–212. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mukarovsky, J. 1932. Standard language and poetic language. In A Prague school reader on esthetics, literary structure, and style, 17–30. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Weather Service. 2009. Naming hurricanes. Retrieved from http://www.nhc.noaa.gov/HAW2/english/basics/naming.shtml.

  • O’Keefe, D.J. 1977. Two concepts of argument. The Journal of the American Forensic Association 13: 121–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silverstein, M. 1979. Language structure and linguistic ideology. In The elements: A parasession on linguistic units and levels, April 20–21, 1979: [proceedings], Conference on Non-Slavic Languages of the USSR, ed. P.R. Clyne, C.L. Hofbauer and W.F. Hanks, 193–247. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silverstein, M. 1985. The functional stratification of language and ontogenesis. In Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives, ed. J.V. Wertsch, 205–235. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silverstein, M. 1993. Metapragmatic discourse and metapragmatic function. In Reflexive language: Reported speech and metapragmatics, 33–58. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, A. 1978. The long road to objectivity and back again: The kinds of truth we get in journalism. In Newspaper history from the seventeenth century to the present day, 153–171. London/Beverly Hills: Constable/Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trew, T. 1979b. Theory and ideology at work. In Language and control, 94–116. London/Boston: Routledge/Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuchman, G. 1980. Making news: A study in the construction of reality. New York/London: Free Press/Collier Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Urban, G. 1996. Entextualization, replication, power. In Natural histories of discourse, ed. M. Silverstein and G. Urban, 21–44. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vendler, Z. 1967. Linguistics in philosophy. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, P. 1997. Death, disruption, and the moral order: The narrative impulse in mass-media hard news reporting. In Genre and institutions: Social processes in the workplace and school, 101–133. London: Cassell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, R.E., and A.L. Becker. 1966. The role of lexical and grammatical cues in paragraph recognition. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan. Retrieved from http://eric.ed.gov/ERICDocs/data/ericdocs2sql/content_storage_01/0000019b/80/33/32/98.pdf.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter A. Cramer .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Cramer, P.A. (2011). Controversy as an Event Category. In: Controversy as News Discourse. Argumentation Library, vol 19. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-1288-1_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics