Skip to main content

Instructional Implications: Toward Visual Thinking in Mathematics

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Toward a Visually-Oriented School Mathematics Curriculum

Part of the book series: Mathematics Education Library ((MELI,volume 49))

  • 1274 Accesses

Abstract

In this closing chapter, I should point out that we certainly have come a long way since the time Klotz (1991) asserted that “visualization has a more important role to play in mathematics education” and the need to be “willing to ask hard questions about this approach” (p. 103). Research data from a variety of sources at least in the last 20 years or so should help us deal with a nagging issue on visualization that Taussig (2009) has succinctly captured for us in the following way: “But to get to basics, why draw?1” (p. 265).

Simply put, the visual teaches us to think with the body.

(Sherwin, Feigenson, & Spiesel, 2007, p. 147)

The advantage of verbal formulations is that they can conform to semantic or logical rules, but a preoccupation with syntactical features of representation means that we still lack an understanding of how visual thinking works in conjunction with language-based reasoning.

(Gooding, 2006, p. 41)

I see the growth of mathematical knowledge as a process in which an unrigorous reasoning-practice, a scattered set of beliefs about manipulations of physical objects, gives rise to a succession of multi-faceted practices through rational transitions, leading ultimately to the mathematics of today.

(Kitcher, 1983, p. 226)

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Taussig’s (2009) reflection about the picture/text hierarchy below is worth noting in light of our goal in this book of suturing the split:

    [T]he hiatus or no-man’s land between picture and text in the anthropological tradition raises a further question as to the general devaluation of drawing in relation to reading and writing in modern Western cultures and maybe in many other cultures as well. We do everything to get children to read, write, and speak well. But why not draw too? Shortly after I wrote this I drove to the supermarket close to where I live in upstate New York past a sign on the road. It read: “Summer Reading Camp.”

    (Taussig, 2009, p. 268; italics added for emphasis)

  2. 2.

    I borrowed these terms from Tucker (2006) who used them in the context of the history of visual representations in science, in particular, nineteenth century scientific photographs. The use of pretty pictures conveys the need to popularize scientific discourses and “the packaging of scientific concepts for mass audiences” (p. 117). By illustration fallacy, it refers to “the mistake of assuming that illustrations produced ‘outside’ of professional science lack scientific significance or value” (p. 117).

  3. 3.

    I have intentionally left out research results from studies involving statistics, geometry, and technology in mathematics learning. But there are interesting overlaps in terms of findings and implications. Statistics and geometry in the school mathematics curriculum fundamentally rely on visual representations, so the visual status in these content strands is not as problematic as the case with the algebra and number sense strands. Certainly, there is much instructional and psychological knowledge that could be gained from research done in these two areas, so I refer readers to exemplary syntheses on geometry understanding (which includes spatial understanding) and statistics learning (e.g., Battista, 2007; Clements & Battista, 1992; Gattis, 2001; Owens & Outhred, 2006; Shaughnessy, 1992, 2007). Concerning technology in mathematics learning, I refer readers to several syntheses of research that also discuss the central role of visual representations in mathematical knowledge acquisition (Ferrara, Pratt, & Robutti, 2006; Laborde, Kynigos, Hollebrands, & Strässer, 2006; Kaput, 1992; Mariotti, 2002; Yerushalmy & Chazan, 2002; Zbiek, Heid, Bume, & Dick, 2007).

  4. 4.

    Gregory (2005) also surfaces the continuous relationship between image and knowledge and construction (i.e., what “it” is and what “it” is for). He writes:

    We see a table as something to support things, and as made of hard scratchable inflammable wood we have learned about from years of interactive experiments. A picture of a table calls up this object-knowledge so it looks almost real – yet pictures are very odd. … When we look at a familiar object we interact with it by appreciating its potential uses or functions. Its picture calls up the knowledge of functions; but at the same time we are warned, by noting that it is merely a picture, not to expect anything materially useful from it.

    (Gregory, 2005, p. 120)

  5. 5.

    I am using the term here in a metaphorical manner. But as an aside I refer readers to Gergen’s (2002) very interesting article in which he explored the phenomenon of absent presence – “present but simultaneously rendered absent … erased by an absent presence” (p. 227) – resulting from our “diverted or divided consciousness invited by communication technology” (p. 227) and cyber-driven culture.

  6. 6.

    The terms generalization, abstraction, and gradual abstracting are conceptually related to the three types of abstracting activity offered by Brook (1997), that is, abstracting out, abstracting away, and building abstraction, respectively.

  7. 7.

    Radford’s (2009) notion of sensuous cognition encompasses the emotional dimension that Peirce has noted about the term sensuous relative to abductive action. As an “alternative approach to classical mental [and rational] views of cognition,” Radford characterizes sensuous modes of thinking in mathematics in “multimodal material” terms, which involve “a sophisticated coordination” of speech and symbols, gestures, body, and actions performed on cultural tools (p. 111) in order to deepen our understanding of how such “ephemeral symptoms [do not merely] announc(e) the imminent arrival of abstract thinking, but genuine constituents of it” (p. 123). A good example of this sensuous cognitive action to pattern generalization is shown in Fig. 5.5. Second grader Dexter explained the structure of his generalization by pointing out the necessary parts using his right hand. In this book, I addressed and emphasized various aspects of what I might classify as visual rational cognitive action in the context of sociocultural learning despite the appearance of similarities in the manner Radford (2009) developed his sensuous cognitive perspective. Both perspectives are complementary, of course. Finally, I note Thagard’s (2010) initial exploration of what he calls embodied abduction that appears to share many of the same features of sensuous cognition. Embodied abduction involves the generation of explanatory hypotheses by a convolutary conceptual process that combines information drawn from two or more modality sources.

References

  • Barmby, P., Harries, T., Higgins, S., & Suggate. J. (2009). The array representation and primary children’s understanding and reasoning in multiplication. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70, 217–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Battista, M. (2007). The development of geometric and spatial thinking. In F. Lester, Jr. (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 843–908). Charlotte, NC: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics and Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bier, C. (2008). Art and Mithãl: Reading geometry as visual commentary. Iranian Studies, 41(4), 491–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brook, A. (1997). Approaches to abstraction: A commentary. International Journal of Educational Research, 27, 77–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cai, J., & Moyer, J. (2008). Developing algebraic thinking in earlier grades: Some insights from international comparative studies. In C. Greenes & R. Rubenstein (Eds.), Algebra and algebraic thinking in school mathematics (pp. 169–182). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clements, D., & Battista, M. (1992). Geometry and spatial reasoning. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 420–464). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davey, N. (2005). Aesthetic f(r)iction: The conflicts of visual experience. Journal of Visual Art Practice, 4(2–3), 135–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deger, J. (2007). Viewing the invisible: Yolngu video as revelatory ritual. Visual Anthropology, 20, 103–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrara, F., Pratt, D., & Robutti, O. (2006). The role and uses of technologies for the teaching of algebra and calculus. In A. Gutierrez & P. Boero (Eds.), Handbook of research on the psychology of mathematics education: Past, present, and future (pp. 237–274). Rotterdam, Netherlands: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gattis, M. (Ed.) (2001). Spatial schemas and abstract thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gee, J. P. (2005). Why video games are good for your soul: Pleasure and learning. Australia: Common Ground Publishing Pty Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., Alibali, M., & Church, R. (1993). Transitions in concept acquisition: Using the hand to read the mind. Psychological Review, 100, 279–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gooding, D. (2006). From phenomenology to field theory: Faraday’s visual reasoning. Perspectives on Science, 14(1), 40–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, N. (1976). Languages of art: An approach to a theory of symbols. Cambridge, MA: Hackett.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, R. (2005). Images of mind in brain. Word & Image, 21(2), 120–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaput, J. (1992). Technology and mathematics education. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 515–556). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klotz, E. (1991). Visualization in geometry: A case study of a multimedia mathematics education project. In W. Zimmermann & S. Cunningham (Eds.), Visualization in teaching and learning mathematics (pp. 95–104). Washington, DC: Mathematical Association of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laborde, C., Kynigos, C., Hollebrands, K., & Strässer, R. (2006). Teaching and learning geometry with technology. In A. Gutierrez & P. Boero (Eds.), Handbook of research on the psychology of mathematics education: Past, present, and future (pp. 275–304). Rotterdam, Netherlands: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics, and culture in everyday life. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Leushina, A. (1991). Soviet studies in mathematics education (volume 4): The development of elementary mathematical concepts in preschool children. (J. Teller, Trans.). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. (1976). Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mariotti, M. A. (2002). The influence of technological advances on students’ mathematics learning. In L. English (Ed.), Handbook of international research in mathematics education (pp. 695–724). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, A. (1997). Cultures of creativity: Mathematics and physics. Diogenes, 45(1), 53–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. (1990). The essence of images. In H. Barlow, C. Blakemore, & M. Weston-Smith (Eds.), Images and understanding (pp. 1–4). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, W. (1994). Picture theory: Essays on verbal and visual representation. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moyer, P. (2001). Are we having fun yet? How teachers use manipulatives to teach mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 47, 175–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mulligan, J., Prescott, A., & Mitchelmore, M. (2003). Taking a closer look at young students’ visual imagery. Australian Primary Mathematics, 8(4), 23–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. (1994). Faraday to Einstein: Constructing meaning in scientific theories. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Netz, R. (1998). Greek mathematical diagrams: Their use and their meaning. For the Learning of Mathematics, 18(3), 33–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Halloran, K. (2005). Mathematical discourse: Language, symbolism, and visual images. New York: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Owens, K., & Outhred, L. (2006). The complexity of learning geometry and measurement. In A. Gutierrez & P. Boero (Eds.), Handbook of research on the psychology of mathematics education: Past, present, and future (pp. 83–115). Rotterdam, Netherlands: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. (1949). In M. Cohen (Ed.) Chance, love, and logic: Philosophical essays. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2009). Why do gestures matter? Sensuous cognition and the palpability of mathematical meanings. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70, 111–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raju, C. K. (2001). Computers, mathematics education, and the alternative epistemology of the calculus in the Yuktibhasa. Philosophy East & West, 51(3), 325–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., & Thom, J. (2009). Bodily experience and mathematical conceptions: From classical views to a phenomenological reconceptualization. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70, 175–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaughnessy, J. M. (1992). Research in probability and statistics: Reflections and directions. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 465–494). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaughnessy, J. M. (2007). Research on statistics learning and reasoning. In F. Lester, Jr. (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 957–1010). Charlotte, NC: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics and Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherwin, R., Feigenson, N., & Spiesel, C. (2007). What is visual knowledge, and what is it good for? Potential ethnographic lessons from the field of legal practice. Visual Anthropology, 20, 143–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, E., & Mevarech, Z. (1996). Children’s understanding of successive divisions in different contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61(2), 153–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taussig, M. (2009). What do drawings want? Culture, Theory, & Critique, 50(2–3), 263–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, J. M. (2006). The historian, the picture, and the archive. Isis, 97, 111–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vergnaud, G. (2009). The theory of conceptual fields. Human Development, 52, 83–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zbiek, R., Heid, M., Bume, G., & Dick, T. (2007). Research on technology in mathematics education: The perspective of constructs. In F. Lester, Jr. (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 1169–1208). Charlotte, NC: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics and Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnheim, R. (1971). Visual thinking. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rivera, F. (2007a). Accounting for students’ schemes in the development of a graphical process for solving polynomial inequalities in instrumented activity. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 65(3), 281–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitcher, P. (1983). The nature of mathematical knowledge. Oxford, MA: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yerushalmy, M., & Chazan, D. (2002). Flux in school algebra: Curricular change, graphing technology, and research on student learning and teacher knowledge. In L. English (Ed.), Handbook of international research on mathematics education (pp. 725–755). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ng, S. F., & Lee, K. (2009). The model method: Singapore children’s tool for representing and solving algebraic word problems. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 40(3), 282–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kulp, M., Earley, M., Mitchell, G., Timmerman, L., Frasco, C., & Geiger, M. (2004). Are visual perceptual skills related to mathematics ability in second through sixth grade children? Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics , 26(4), 44–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergen, K. (2002). The challenge of absent presence. In J. Katz & M. Aakhus (Eds.), Perceptual contact: Mobile communication, private talk, public performance (pp. 227–241). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tiragallo, F. (2007). Embodiment of the gaze: Vision, planning, and weaving between filmic ethnography and cultural technology. Visual Anthropology, 20, 201–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wise, M. N. (2006). Making visible. Isis, 97, 75–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thagard, P. (2010). How brains make mental models. In L. Magnani, W. Carnielli, & C. PIzzi (Eds.), Model-based reasoning in science and technology: Abduction, logic, and computational discovery (pp. 447–461). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ferdinand D. Rivera .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Rivera, F.D. (2011). Instructional Implications: Toward Visual Thinking in Mathematics. In: Toward a Visually-Oriented School Mathematics Curriculum. Mathematics Education Library, vol 49. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-0014-7_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics