Advertisement

Mind and Society

  • Mario Bunge
Chapter
Part of the Boston Studies in the Philosophy of Science book series (BSPS, volume 287)

Abstract

Immanuel Kant and the Baron Thiry d’Holbach were born in Germany just one year apart at the start of the Enlightenment. If Kant had lived in sparkling Paris rather than in Königsberg, and d’Holbach had stayed in dark Edsheim, his native town, they might have exchanged philosophies: Kant might have become the great materialist and realist philosopher of the century, and d’Holbach his idealist counterpart. Of course, the previous sentence is a counterfactual, and as such untestable, and therefore neither true nor false. But it is not a ludicrous fantasy, because we know that nurture and opportunity are just as important as nature.

Keywords

Episodic Memory Lead Exposure Evolutionary Psychology Mirror Neuron Social Stimulus 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Reference

  1. Cacioppo, John T., Penny S. Visser, and Cynthia I. Pickett, eds. 2006. Social neuroscience: People thinking about thinking people. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  2. Carreiras, Manuel, Mohamed L. Seghier, Silvia Baquero, Adelina Estévez, Alfonso Lozano, Joseph T. Devlin, and Cathy J. Price. 2009. An anatomical signature for literacy. Nature 461: 983–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Chapman, H. A., D. A. Kim, J. M. Susskind, and A. K. Anderson. 2009. In bad taste: Evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science 323: 1222–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Enfield, N. J., and Stephen C. Levinson, eds. 2006. Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction. Oxford and New York: Berg.Google Scholar
  5. Fehr, Ernst, Helen Bernhard, and Bettina Rockenbach. 2008. Egalitarianism in young children. Nature 454: 1079–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Gintis, Herbert, Samuel Bowles, Robert Boyd, and Ernst Fehr, eds. 2005. Moral sentiments and material interests: The foundations of cooperation in economic life. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  7. Henrich, J., R. Boyd, S. Bowles, C. Camerer, E. Fehr, and H. Gintis, eds. 2004. Foundations of human sociality: Economic experiments and ethnographic evidence from fifteen small-scale societies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Kishiyama, Mark M., W. Thomas Boyce, Amy M. Jimenez, Lee M. Perry, and Robert T. Knight. 2009. Socioeconomic disparities affect prefrontal function in children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21: 1106–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. McGowan, Patrick O., Aya Sasaki, Ana C. D. D’Alessio, Sergiy Dymov, Benoit Labonté, Moshe Szyf, Gustavo Turecki, and Michael J. Meaney. (2009) Epigenetic regulation of the glucocorticoid receptor in human brain associates with childhood abuse. Nature Neuroscience 12: 342–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Penn, Derek C., Keith J. Holyoak, and Daniel J. Povinelli. 2008. Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31: 109–78.Google Scholar
  11. Wright, John Paul, Kim N. Dietrich, M. Douglas Ris, Richard W. Hornung, Stephanie D. Wessel, Bruce P. Lanphear, Mona Ho, and Mary N. Rae. 2008. Association of prenatal and childhood blood lead concentrations with criminal arrests in early adulthood. PLOS Medicine 5: 0732–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Medawar, Peter. 1957. The uniqueness of the individual. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  13. Dunbar, R[obin]. I. M. 2003. The social brain: Mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology 32: 163–81.Google Scholar
  14. –––––. 2008. Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  15. Karmiloff-Smith, Annette. 2006. The tortuous route from genes to behavior: A neuroconstructivist approach. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 6: 9–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Loeber, Rolf, and Dustin Pardini. 2008. Neurobiology and the development of violence: common assumptions and controversies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society 363: 2491–503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Mameli, Matteo. 2008. On innateness. Journal of Philosophy 105: 719–36.Google Scholar
  18. Platt, Michael L., and Elizabeth S. Spelke. 2009. What can developmental and comparative cognitive neuroscience tell us abut the adult human brain? Current Opinion in Neurobiology 19: 1–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Vygotsky, L[ev]. S[emyonovich]. 1978. Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Luria, Alexander R.1976. Cognitive development. Its cultural and social foundations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Humphrey, Nicholas. 1983. Consciousness regained. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  22. Cole, Michael. 1996. Cultural psychology: The once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Wolf, Arthur P. 1995. Sexual attraction and childhood association. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Rizzolatti, Giacomo, and Laila Craighero. 2004. The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience 27: 169–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Rizzolatti, Giacomo, and Corrado Sinigaglia. 2008. Mirrors in the brain – How our minds share actions and emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Appiah, Kwame Anthony. 2008. Experiments in ethics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Thaler, Richard H. 1992. The winner’s curse: Paradoxes and anomalies of economic life. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Goodale, Melvyn, and David Milner. (2005). Sight unseen. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Barkow, Jerome H., Leda Cosmides, and John Tooby, eds. 1992. The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  30. Bjorklund, David F., and Anthony D. Pellegrini. 2002. The origins of human nature: Evolutionary developmental psychology. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  31. Tulving, Endel. 2002. Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology 53: 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Gottlieb, Gilbert. 1992. Individual development and evolution: The genesis of novel behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Lorenz, Konrad. 1971 [1954]. Psychology and phylogeny. In Studies in Animal and Human Behavior, vol II, 196–245. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  34. –––––. 1978. Natural selection and the evolution of mind. Dialectica 32: 339–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. –––––. 1987. What evolutionary epistemology is not. In Evolutionary epistemology, ed. W. Callebaut and R. Pinxten, 203–21. Dordrecht, NL: D. Reidel.Google Scholar
  36. –––––. 1972. Objective knowledge: An evolutionary approach. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  37. Smail, Daniel Lord. 2008. On deep history and the brain. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  38. Cavalli-Sforza, Luigi L., and Marcus W. Feldman. 1981. Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Keyfitz, Nathan, ed. 1984. Population and biology: Bridge between disciplines. Liège: Ordina Editions.Google Scholar
  40. Cochran, Gregory, and Thomas Harpending. 2009. The 10,000 year explosion: How civilization accelerated human evolution. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  41. –––––. 1996. The prehistory of the mind. London: Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
  42. Preuss, Todd M. 2007. Primate brain evolution in phylogenetic context. In Evolution of nervous systems, vol. 4: Primates, ed. J. H, Kaas and T. M. Preuss, 1–34. Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  43. De Waal, Frans. 1996. Good natured: The origin of right and wrong. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Rumbaugh, Duane M., and Timothy V. Gill. 1976. The mastery of language-type skills by the chimpanzee (Pan). Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 280: 562–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Adolphs, Ralph. 2009. The social brain: Neural basis of social knowledge. Annual Reviews of Psychology 60: 693–716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Gazzaniga, Michael S. 2008. Human: The science behind what makes us unique. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
  47. Passingham, Richard. 2008. What is special about the human brain? Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  48. Calvin, William H., and Derek Bickerton. 2000. Lingua ex Machina. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  49. Allman, John Morgan. 1999. Evolving brains. New York: Scientific American Library.Google Scholar
  50. Genty, Emilie, and Jean-Jacques Roeder. 2006. Self-control: why should sea lions, Zalophus californianus, perform better than primates? Animal Behavior 72: 1241–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Tomasello, Michael. 2006. Why don’t apes point? In ed. Enfield and Levinson, 506–24.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Dept. of PhilosophyMcGill UniversityMontrealCanada

Personalised recommendations