Skip to main content
  • 933 Accesses

Abstract

About 15 years ago, Robbie asked me what I thought of a talk we had just heard. I indicated that I hadn’t much liked it and noted several serious problems. Robbie agreed with all of the criticisms, but said that he nonetheless liked the talk, because there was one good idea in it that he could use. I agreed with him that the idea was a good one, but it took me a while to understand the wisdom of his position. If there’s one useful idea in a talk, then hearing it has been worthwhile, even if the talk also has numerous deficiencies. On that day and on many others, talking with Robbie changed my thinking for the better.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Capodilupo, A. M. (1992). A neo-structural analysis of children’s response to instruction in the sight-reading of musical notation. In R. Case (Ed.), The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge (pp. 99–116). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1978). Intellectual development from birth to adulthood: A new-Piagetian investigation. In R. S. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops? Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1992). The role of the frontal lobes in the regulation of cognitive development. Brain and Cognition, 20, 51–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1992). The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., & Globerson, T. (1974). Field independence and central computing space. Child Development, 45, 772–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., Hayward, S., Lewis, M., & Hurst, P. (1988). Toward a neo-Piagetian theory of cognitive and emotional development. Developmental Review, 8, 1–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., & Khanna, F. (1981). The missing links: Stages in children’s progression from sensorimotor to logical thought. In K. W. Fischer (Ed.), Cognitive development (New Directions for Child Development, No. 12). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., & McKeough, A. (1990). Schooling and the development of central conceptual structures: An example from the domain of children’s narrative. International Journal of Educational Psychology, 8, 835–855.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., & Okamoto, Y. (1996). The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children’s thought. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61(1–2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., & Sandieson, R. (1992). Testing for the presence of a central quantative structure: Use of the transfer paradigm. In R. Case (Ed.), The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge (pp. 117–132). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crammond, J. (1992). Analyzing the basic cognitive-developmental processes of children with specific types of learning disability. In R. Case (Ed.), The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge (pp. 285–302). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demetriou, A., Efklides, A., & Platsidou, M. (1993). The architecture and dynamics of developing mind: Experiential structuralism as a frame for unifying cognitive developmental theories. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(5–6, Serial No. 234).

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, S. (1992). Stage and structure in the development of children’s spatial representations. In R. Case (Ed.), The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge (pp. 229–246). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiati, T. A. (1992). Cross-cultural variation in the structure of children’s thought. In R. Case (Ed.), The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge (pp. 319–342). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg-Reitman, J. (1992). Young girls’ conception of their mothers’ role: A neo-structural analysis. In R. Case (Ed.), The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge (pp. 135–152). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, S., & Case, R. (1996). Evaluating the breadth and depth of training effects when central conceptual structures are taught. In R. Case & Y. Okamoto (Eds.), The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children’s thought. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61(1–2), 83–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, S., & Case, R. (1997). Rethinking the primary school math curriculum: An approach based on cognitive science. Issues in Education, 3, 1–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, S., Case, R., & Siegler, R. S. (1994). Rightstart: Providing the central conceptual prerequisites for first formal learning of arithmetic to students at risk for school failure. In K. McGilly (Ed.), Classroom lessons: Integrating cognitive theory and classroom practice (pp. 25–49). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halford, G. S. (1993). Children’s understanding: The development of mental models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalchman, M., Moss, J., & Case, R. (2001). Psychological models for the development of mathematical understanding: Rational numbers and functions. In S. M. Carver & D. Klahr (Eds.), Cognition and instruction: Twenty-five years of progress (pp. 1–38). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marini, Z. A. (1992). Synchrony and asynchrony in the development of children’s scientific reasoning. In R. Case (Ed.), The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge (pp. 55–74). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moss, J., & Case, R. (1999). Developing children’s understanding of rational numbers: A new model and experimental curriculum. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30, 122–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Okamoto, Y., & Case, R. (1996). Exploring the microstructure of children’s central conceptual structures in the domain of number. In R. Case & Y. Okamoto (Eds.), The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children’s thought. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61(1–2, Serial No. 246), 27–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Okamoto, Y., Case, R., Bleiker, C., & Henderson, B. (1996). Cross cultural investigations. In R. Case & Y. Okamoto (Eds.), The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children’s thought. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61(1–2, Serial No. 246), 131–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1969). Cognitive development and cognitive style. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Geneva.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porath, M. (1992). Stage and structure in the development of children with various types of “giftedness”. In R. Case (Ed.), The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge (pp. 303–318). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reid, T. R. (1992). Horizontal and vertical structure: Stages and substages in children’s motor development. In R. Case (Ed.), The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge (pp. 247–266). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1996). Emerging minds: The process of change in children’s thinking. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert S. Siegler .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Siegler, R.S. (2010). Introduction. In: Ferrari, M., Vuletic, L. (eds) The Developmental Relations among Mind, Brain and Education. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-3666-7_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics