Advertisement

Mutual Gaze and Intersubjectivity

  • Beata Stawarska
Chapter

Abstrct

In this paper I will address the role of mutual gaze in social cognition. The exposition will run in two steps. First, I propose to examine some recent studies of joint visual attention in order to substantiate the view that social cognition is operative in infancy prior to the emergence of theoretical skills required to make judgements about other people’s states of mind. Such social cognition does not depend on intellectual procedures but rather on the communicative potential inherent in human bodies, for example, the ability to directly engage with others via mutual gaze. This view of sociality as dependent on bodily practices is broadly consistent with the phenomenological philosophy of Merleau-Ponty, which I propose to address second. Specifically, I will examine vision in the context of reversible dynamics which Merleau-Ponty believes regulate intercorporeal relations. This will allow me to expose some inner difficulties within Merleau-Ponty’s position as well as to point out the ways of resolving them by means of combined insights from developmental psychology and the analyses of interpersonal connectedness drawn from the dialogic tradition in philosophy.

Keywords

Social Cognition Person Pronoun False Belief Task Autistic Individual Social Observation 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Argyle M, Cook M (1976) Gaze and mutual gaze. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  2. Barbaras R (2001) De l’être du phénomène. Sur l’ontologie de Merleau-Ponty. Millon, GrenobleGoogle Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen S (2001) Mindblindness. An essay on autism and theory of mind. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  4. Benveniste E (1971) Problems in general linguistics. University of Miami Press, Coral GablesGoogle Scholar
  5. Buber M (1958) I and thou. Scribners, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  6. Gomez JC (1991) Visual behavior as a window for reading the mind of others in primates. In: Whiten A (ed) Natural theories of mind. Blackwell, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  7. Gomez JC, Sarriá E, Tamarit J (1993) The comparative study of early communication and theories of mind: ontogeny, phylogeny, and pathology. In: Baron Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen D (eds) Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  8. Gomez JC (1994) Mutual awareness: a Gricean approach. In: Parker ST, Mietchell RW, Boccia ML (eds) Self-awareness in animals and humans. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  9. Hobson P (2004) The cradle of thought. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  10. Hobson P (1997) Are there “autistic-like” features in congenitally blind children? J Child Psychol 38(6):693-703CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hull JM (2001) On sight and insight. Oneworld Publications, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  12. Jopling D (1993) The philosophy of dialogue. In: Neisser U (ed) The perceived self. Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  13. Lyons J (1977) Semantics, vol I & II. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  14. Merleau-Ponty M (2003) La Nature. Northwestern University Press, EvanstonGoogle Scholar
  15. Merleau-Ponty M (2000) The prose of the world. Northwestern University Press, EvanstonGoogle Scholar
  16. Merleau-Ponty M (1968) The visible and the invisible. Northwestern University Press, EvanstonGoogle Scholar
  17. Merleau-Ponty M (1964a) The philosopher and his shadow. Signs. Northwestern University Press, EvanstonGoogle Scholar
  18. Merleau-Ponty M (1964b) Eye and Mind. The primacy of perception and other essays. Northwestern University Press, EvanstonGoogle Scholar
  19. Overgaard S (2006) The problem of other minds: Wittgenstein’s phenomenological perspective. Phenomenol Cogn Sci 5(1):53-73Google Scholar
  20. Robson KS (1967) The role of eye-to-eye contact in maternal-infant attachment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 8(1):13-25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Rochat P (2001) The infant’s world. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA/LondonGoogle Scholar
  22. Sartre JP (1956) Being and nothingness. An essay on phenomenological ontology. Philosophical Library, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  23. Schutz A (1967) The phenomenology of the social world. Northwestern University Press, EvanstonGoogle Scholar
  24. Stawarska B (2004) The body, the mirror and the other in Merleau-Ponty and Sartre. In: Gallagher S, Watson S (eds) Ipseity and alterity: interdisciplinary approaches to intersubjectivity. Presses Universitaires de Rouen, RouenGoogle Scholar
  25. Stawarska B (2002) Reversibility and intersubjectivity in Merleau-Ponty’s ontology. J Br Soc Phenomenol 33(2):155-166Google Scholar
  26. Stern DN (1974) Mother and infant at play: the dyadic interaction involving facial, vocal and gaze behaviors. In: Lewis M, Rosenblum LA (eds) The effect of the infant on its caregiver. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  27. Varela FJ, Thompson E, Rosch E (2000) The embodied mind. Cognitive science and human experience. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  28. Wimmer H, Perner J (1983) Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 13:103-128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Wolff PH (1963) Observations of the early development in smiling. In: Foss BM (ed) Determinants of infant behavior, vol II. Wiley, New York, pp 113-138Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  • Beata Stawarska
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PhilosophyUniversity of OregonEugeneUSA

Personalised recommendations