Advertisement

Empirical and Phenomenological Studies of Embodied Cognition

  • David Morris
Chapter

Abstrct

The first grand wave of twentieth century cognitive science and philosophy of mind sought and still seeks to show how, contra Cartesian dualism, cognition is materialized in the brain. A new wave is rising from this, though, one that studies cognition not merely as “embrained” (Damasio 2000, 118; Collins 2000) but as embodied in a much stronger sense, as inseparable from and shaped by the concrete extra-cerebral structures and dynamics of the body, and the body’s embeddedness in the natural and social world. This wave is gathering momentum. Whereas a decade ago researchers needed to protest that cognition is in fact strongly embodied, one can now find studies that leap right to the problem of how and in what sense it is so. This is a telltale sign of a nascent paradigm shift. Yet, in studying this new wave one finds that philosophy and science are still - for reasons recently remarked (see Dreyfus 2007; Gallagher 2007) and soon discussed - at sea for lack of conceptual frameworks to bring embodied cognition back to firm land. Brooks, whose robotics research is part of the new wave, puts it succinctly: “perhaps at this point we simply do not get it, and there is some fundamental change necessary in our thinking.” (1991; cited in Dreyfus 2007, 251)

Keywords

Perceptual Cognition Cognitive Work Cartesian Dualism Telltale Sign Representational Relation 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Amorim M-A, Isableu B (2006) Embodied spatial transformations: “body analogy” for the mental rotation of objects. J Exp Psychol Gen 135:327-347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson M (2007) Review of how the body shapes the mind. Philos Psychol 20:127-142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barbaras R (1992) Motricité et phénoménalité chez le dernier Merleau-Ponty. In: Richir M, Tassin E (eds) Merleau-Ponty: phénoménologie et expériences. Jérôme Millon, GrenobleGoogle Scholar
  4. Barbaras R (2003) Life and perceptual intentionality. Res Phenomenol 33:157-166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Berthoz A (2000) The brain’s sense of movement. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  6. Berthoz A (2006) Emotion and reason: the cognitive science of decision making (trans: Giselle W). Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  7. Bongard JC, Paul C (2000) Investigating morphological symmetry and locomotive efficiency using virtual embodied evolution. In: Meyer J-A, Berthoz A, Floreano D, Roitblat H, Wilson SW (eds) From animals to animats 6: Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Simulation of Adaptive Behavior. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 420-429Google Scholar
  8. Borrett D, Kelly S, Kwan H (2000) Phenomenology, dynamical neural networks and brain function. Philos Psychol 13(2):213-228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brooks R (1991) Intelligence without representation. Artif Int 47:139-159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Burwood S, Gilbert P, Lennon K (1999) Philosophy of mind. McGill-Queen’s University Press, MontrealGoogle Scholar
  11. Casey ES (2000) Remembering: a phenomenological study, 2nd edn. Indiana University Press, Bloomington, INGoogle Scholar
  12. Churchland PS, Ramachandran VS, Sejnowski TJ (1994) A critique of pure vision. In: Koch C, Davis JL (eds) Large scale neuronal theories of the brain: computational neuroscience. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 23-60Google Scholar
  13. Clark A (2001) Reasons, robots, and the extended mind. Mind Lang 16(2):121-145Google Scholar
  14. Clark A (2006) Material symbols. Philos Psychol 19:291-307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Clark A, Chalmers DJ (1998) The extended mind. Analysis 58:7-19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Collins HM (2000) Four kinds of knowledge, two (or maybe three) kinds of embodiment, and the question of artificial intelligence. In: Wrathall M, Malpas J (eds) Heidegger, coping, and cognitive science: essays in honor of Hubert L. Dreyfus. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 179-195Google Scholar
  17. Collins S, Ruina A, Tedrake R, Wisse M (2005) Efficient bipedal robots based on passive-dynamic walkers. Science 307:1082-1085CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Csepregi G (2006) The clever body. The University of Calgary Press, CalgaryGoogle Scholar
  19. Damasio AR (1999) The feeling of what happens: body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Harcourt Brace, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  20. Damasio AR (2000) Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. Quill, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  21. Damasio AR (2003) Looking for spinoza: joy, sorrow, and the feeling brain. Harcourt, Orlando, FLGoogle Scholar
  22. Dewey J (1972) The reflex arc concept in psychology. In: Axetell GE, Boydston, JA (ed) The early works, 1882-1898. Southern Illinois University Press, Carbondale, pp 96-109Google Scholar
  23. Dickinson MH, Farley CT, Full RJ, Koehl MAR, Kram R, Lehman S (2000) How animals move: an integrative view. Science 288:100-106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Djikstra K, Michael PK, Rolf AZ (2007) Body posture facilitates retrieval of autobiographical memories. Cognition 102:139-149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Dreyfus HL (1991) Being-in-the-world: a commentary on Heidegger’s being and time, Division I. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  26. Dreyfus HL (1992) What computers still can’t do: a critique of artificial reason. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  27. Dreyfus HL (1998) The current relevance of Merleau-Ponty’s phenomenology of embodiment http://ejap.louisiana.edu/EJAP/1996.spring/dreyfus.1996.spring.abs.html
  28. Dreyfus HL (2002) Intelligence without representation - Merleau-Ponty’s critique of mental representation: the relevance of phenomenology to scientific explanation. Phenomenol Cogn Sci 1:367-383CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Dreyfus HL (2007) Why Heideggerian AI failed and how fixing it would require making it more Heideggerian. Philos Psychol 20(2):247-268CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Dreyfus HL, Hall H (1982) Husserl, intentionality, and cognitive science. MIT Press/Bradford Books, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  31. Fogel A (1993) Developing through relationships: origins of communication, self, and culture. Harvester, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  32. Freeman WJ (1999) How brains make up their mind. Weidenfeld & Nicolson, LondonGoogle Scholar
  33. Full RJ, Farley CT (2000) Musculoskeletal dynamics in rhythmic systems: a comparative approach to legged locomotion. In: Winters JM, Crago PE (eds) Biomechanics and neural control of posture and movement. Springer Verlag, New York, pp 192-205Google Scholar
  34. Galison P (1997) Image and logic: a material culture of microphysics. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, ILGoogle Scholar
  35. Gallagher S (1986a) Body image and body schema: a conceptual clarification. J Mind Behav 7:541-554Google Scholar
  36. Gallagher S (1986b) Hyletic experience and the lived body. Husserl Stud 3:131-166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Gallagher S (1986c) Lived body and environment. Res Phenomenol 16:139-170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Gallagher S (1995) Body schema and intentionality. In: Bermúdez JL, Marcel A, Eilan N (eds) The body and the self. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 225-244Google Scholar
  39. Gallagher S (1997) Mutual enlightenment: recent phenomenology in cognitive science. J Conscious Stud 4(3):195-214Google Scholar
  40. Gallagher S (2004) Understanding interpersonal problems in autism: interaction theory as an alternative to theory of mind. Philos Psychiat Psychol 11:199-217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Gallagher S (2005) How the body shapes the mind. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Gallagher S (2007) Review of Mark Rowlands, body language: representation in action. In: Notre Dame philosophical reviews http://ndpr.nd.edu/; the review is dated 2007.09.19, and is at http://ndpr.nd.edu/review.cfm?id=11183
  43. Gallagher S, Marcel AJ (1999) The self in contextualized action. In: Gallagher S, Shear J (eds) Models of the self. Imprint Academic, Thorverton, UK, pp 273-299Google Scholar
  44. Gibbs RW (2006) Embodiment and cognitive science. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  45. Gibbs RW, Berg EA (2000) Mental imagery and embodied activity. J Ment Imagery 26:1-30Google Scholar
  46. Groopman JE (2007) How doctors think. Houghton Mifflin, Boston, MAGoogle Scholar
  47. Heidegger M (1962) Being and time (trans: Macquarrie J, Robinson E). Harper & Row, New York. Original edition 1927Google Scholar
  48. Hurley SL (1998) Consciousness in action. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  49. Husserl E (1970) The crisis of the European sciences and transcendental phenomenology (trans:Carr D). Northwestern University Press, Evanston, ILGoogle Scholar
  50. Husserl E (1989) Ideas pertaining to a pure phenomenology and to a phenomenological philosophy. Second book: Studies in the phenomenology of constitution (trans: Rojcewicz R, Schuwer A) Kluwer, DordrechtGoogle Scholar
  51. Husserl E (1991) Cartesian meditations (trans: Cairns D). Kluwer, Dordrecht. Original edition 1931Google Scholar
  52. James W (1950) The principles of psychology, vol 1. Dover, New York. Original edition 1890Google Scholar
  53. Johnson M (1987) The body in the mind: the bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, ILGoogle Scholar
  54. Johnson M (2007) The meaning of the body. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, ILGoogle Scholar
  55. Katz D (1989) The world of touch. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJGoogle Scholar
  56. Kermoian R, Campos JJ (1988) Locomotor experience: a facilitator of spatial and cognitive development. Child Dev 59:908-917CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Kubzansky PE (1961) The effects of reduced environmental stimulation on human behavior: a review. In: Biderman A, Zimmer H (eds) The manipulation of human behavior. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  58. Lakoff G, Johnson M (1999) Philosophy in the flesh: the embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. Basic Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  59. Lakoff G, Núñez RE (2000) Where mathematics comes from: how the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. Basic Books, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  60. Landgrebe L (1973) The phenomenological concept of experience. Philos Phenomenol Res 34(1):1-13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Leder D (1990) The absent body. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, ILGoogle Scholar
  62. Leder D (1992) A tale of two bodies: the cartesian corpse and the lived body. In: Leder D (ed) The body in medical thought and practice. Kluwer, DordrechtGoogle Scholar
  63. Lederman SJ, Klatzky RL (1987) Hand movements: a window into haptic object recognition. Cogn Psychol 19:54-64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Lee M-W, Gibbons J (2007) Rhythmic alternation and the optional complementiser in english: new evidence of phonological influence on grammatical encoding. Cognition 105:446-456CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Lloyd DE (2004) Radiant cool: a novel theory of consciousness. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  66. McNeill D (1992) Hand and mind: what gestures reveal about thought. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, ILGoogle Scholar
  67. Merleau-Ponty M (1962) Phenomenology of perception (trans: Colin S). Humanities, Atlantic Highlands, New Jersey. Original edition 1945Google Scholar
  68. Merleau-Ponty M (1964a) The child’s relation with others. In: Edie JM (ed) The primacy of perception. Northwestern University Press, Evanston, IL, pp 96-155Google Scholar
  69. Merleau-Ponty M (1964b) The primacy of perception and its philosophical consequences. In The primacy of perception. Northwestern University Press, Evanston, IL, pp 12-42Google Scholar
  70. Merleau-Ponty M (1965) The structure of behaviour (trans: Fisher AL). Methuen, London. Original edition 1941Google Scholar
  71. Merleau-Ponty M (2002) Husserl at the limits of phenomenology. Northwestern University Press, Evanston, ILGoogle Scholar
  72. Noë A (2004) Action in perception, representation and mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  73. O’Regan JK, Noë A (2001a) A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behav Brain Sci 24:939-1031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. O’Regan JK, Noë A (2001b) What it is like to see: a sensorimotor theory of perceptual experience. Synthese 129:79-103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Olafson FA (1987) Heidegger and the philosophy of mind. Yale University Press, New Haven, CTGoogle Scholar
  76. Petit J-L (2003) On the relation between recent neurobiological data on perception (and action) and the Husserlian theory of constitution. Phenomenol Cogn Sci 2:281-298CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Philips-Silver J, Trainor LJ (2007) Hearing what the body feels: auditory encoding of rhythmic movement. Cognition 105:533-546CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Roth W-M, Lawless D (2002) How does the body get into the mind? Hum Stud 25:333-358CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Russon J (2003) Human experience: philosophy, neurosis, and the elements of everyday life. State University of New York Press, AlbanyGoogle Scholar
  80. Russon J (2006) On human identity: the intersubjective path from body to mind. Dialogue: Can Philos Rev XLV:306-314Google Scholar
  81. Sheets-Johnstone M (1990) The roots of thinking. Temple University Press, PhiladelphiaGoogle Scholar
  82. Sheets-Johnstone M (1999a) Phenomenology and agency: methodological and theoretical issues in Strawson’s ‘The self’. In: Gallagher S, Shear J (eds) Models of the self. Imprint Academic, Thorverton, UK, pp 231-252Google Scholar
  83. Sheets-Johnstone M (1999b) The primacy of movement. John Benjamins Publishing Company, AmsterdamGoogle Scholar
  84. Sheets-Johnstone M (1999c) Re-thinking Husserl’s fifth meditation. Philos Today SPEP Suppl:99-106Google Scholar
  85. Sheets-Johnstone M (2003a) Kinesthetic memory. Theoria et Historia Scientiarum: Int J Interdis Stud 7:69-92Google Scholar
  86. Sheets-Johnstone M (2003b) Movement: the generative source of spatial perception and cognition. Francine Dolins and Robert Mitchell, (eds.). Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  87. Thelen E (1984) Learning to walk: ecological demands and phylogenetic constraints. Adv Infancy Res 3:213-250Google Scholar
  88. Thelen E (1995) Motor development: a new synthesis. Am Psychol 50(2):79-95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Thelen E (2000) Grounded in the world: developmental origins of the embodied mind. Infancy 1(1):3-28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Thelen E, Smith LB (1994) A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. In: Palmer S (ed) MIT Press/Bradford Books series in cognitive psychology. MIT Press/Bradford Books, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  91. Thompson E (1997) Symbol grounding: a bridge from artificial life to artificial intelligence. Brain Cogn 34:48-71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Thompson E (2007) Mind in life: biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  93. Tucker DM (2007) Mind from body: experience from neural structure. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  94. Turvey MT (2007) Action and perception at the level of synergies. Hum Mov Sci 26:657-697CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Varela FJ (1991) Organism: a meshwork of selfless selves. In: Tauber AI (ed) Organism and the origins of self. Kluwer, Dordrecht, The Netherlands, pp 79-107Google Scholar
  96. Varela FJ (1996) Neurophenomenology: a methodological remedy for the hard problem. J Conscious Stud 3(4):330-349Google Scholar
  97. Varela FJ (1999) The specious present: a neurophenomenology of time consciousness. In: Petitot J, Varela FJ, Pachoud B, Roy J-M (eds) Naturalizing phenomenology: issues in contemporary phenomenology and cognitive science. Stanford University Press, Stanford, pp 266-314Google Scholar
  98. Varela FJ, Thompson E, Rosch E (1993) The embodied mind: cognitive science and human experience. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  99. Wheeler M (2005) Reconstructing the cognitive world: the next step. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  100. Wilson M (2002) Six views of embodied cognition. Psychon Bull Rev 9(4):625-636Google Scholar
  101. Wrathall M, Malpas J (eds) (2000) Heidegger, coping, and cognitive science. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  102. Zahavi D (1994) Husserl’s phenomenology of the body. Etudes Phénoménologique 19:63-84Google Scholar
  103. Zahavi D (2003) Husserl’s phenomenology. Stanford University Press, Stanford, CAGoogle Scholar
  104. Zelazo PR (1983) The development of walking: new findings and old assumptions. J Motor Behav 15:99-137Google Scholar
  105. Zelazo PR (1984) Learning to walk: recognition of higher order influences. Adv Infancy Res 3:251-260Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  • David Morris
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PhilosophyConcordia UniversityMontrealCanada

Personalised recommendations