Skip to main content

Psychological and Neural Functions of Working Memory in Social Contexts

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Memory in a Social Context

Abstract

In everyday life, we need to temporally maintain and manipulate various pieces of information in our mind to achieve social and interpersonal goals. Such a temporal memory system that supports goal-directed behavior is called working memory. This article reviews how working memory functions in social contexts from both a psychological and a neuroscience perspective. Many neuroimaging studies have revealed that the frontoparietal network (FPN) is activated when working memory keeps information available independent of the information modality and that the default mode network (DMN) is deactivated when working memory is employed. Thus, the FPN and DMN often show an inverse pattern of activation. Psychological studies investigating the contributions of working memory to mind reading or “theory of mind” (ToM) show that working memory plays a role in keeping available information that is necessary for reasoning about the mental states of others. Therefore, a psychological perspective assumes that FPN activation accompanied by DMN deactivation might support ToM. However, the DMN is also involved in representing self-generated thoughts that are not perceived by our senses, including the mental states of others. Indeed, the DMN includes a set of brain regions that are indispensable to social cognition. Therefore, a neuroscience perspective enables us to assume that balancing and switching between the FPN, which is associated with working memory, and the DMN, which is associated with representing social information, might optimize social cognition. Finally, anticipated avenues for future research on working memory in social cognition are suggested.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Allen M, Smallwood J, Christensen J, Gramm D, Rasmussen B, Jensen CG, Roepstorff A, Lutz A (2013) The balanced mind: the variability of task-unrelated thoughts predicts error monitoring. Front Hum Neurosci 7:743

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Amodio DM, Frith CD (2006) Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 7(4):268–277

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley AD (1986) Working memory. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley AD (2000) The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn Sci 4(11):417–423

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley AD (2007) Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley AD (2012) Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annu Rev Psychol 63:1–29

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ (1974) Working memory. In: Bower GH (ed) The psychology of learning and motivation, vol 8. Academic Press, New York, pp 47–89

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U (1985) Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition 21(1):37–46

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Lombardo MV (eds) (2013) Understanding other minds: perspective from developmental social neuroscience. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Baune BT, Fuhr M, Air T, Hering C (2014) Neuropsychological functioning in adolescents and young adults with major depressive disorder: a review. Psychiatry Res 218(3):261–271

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Braver TS, Cohen JD, Nystrom LE, Jonides J, Smith EE, Noll DC (1997) A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory. NeuroImage 5(1):49–62

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brouillet P, Portrat S, Camos V (2011) On the law relating processing to storage in working memory. Psychol Rev 118(2):175–192

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL (2008) The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1124:1–38

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bull R, Phillips LH, Conway CA (2008) The role of control functions in mentalizing: dual-task studies of theory of mind and executive function. Cogn, 107(2):663–672

    Google Scholar 

  • Bzdok D, Schilbach L, Vogeley K, Schneider K, Laird AR, Langner R, Eickhoff SB (2012) Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta-analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy. Brain Struct Funct 217(4):783–796

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson SM, Moses LJ, Breton C (2002) How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant Child Dev 11(2):73–92

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrington SJ, Bailey AJ (2009) Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature. Hum Brain Mapp 30(8):2313–2335

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cavanna AE, Trimble MR (2006) The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129(3):564–583

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christoff K, Gordon AM, Smallwood J, Smith R, Schooler JW (2009) Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106(21):8719–8724

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL (2008) The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58(3):306–324

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Costa A, Torriero S, Oliveri M, Caltagirone C (2008) Prefrontal and temporo–parietal involvement in taking others’ perspective: TMS evidence. Behav Neurol 19(1–2):71–74

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cowan N (2005) Working memory capacity (Essays in cognitive psychology). Psychology Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Crone EA, Wendelken C, Donohue S, van Leijenhorst L, Bunge SA (2006) Neurocognitive development of the ability to manipulate information in working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103(24):9315–9320

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis CE, D’Esposito M (2003) Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends Cogn Sci 7(9):415–423

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • D’Esposito M, Detre JA, Alsop DC, Shin RK, Atlas S, Grossman M (1995) The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature 378(6554):279–281

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Daneman M, Merikle PM (1996) Working memory and language comprehension: a meta–analysis. Psychon Bull Rev 3(4):422–433

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis HL, Pratt C (1995) The development of children’s theory of mind: the working memory explanation. Aust J Psychol 47(1):25–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis M, Agostino A, Roncadin C, Levin HS (2009) Theory of mind depends on domain–general executive functions of working memory and cognitive inhibition in children with traumatic brain injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 31(7):835–847

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Druzgal TJ, D’Esposito M (2001) Activity in fusiform face area modulated as a function of working memory load. Cogn Brain Res 10(3):355–364

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ellamil M, Dobson C, Beeman M, Christoff K (2012) Evaluative and generative modes of thought during the creative process. NeuroImage 59(2):1783–1794

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher HL, Frith CD (2003) Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends Cogn Sci 7(2):77–83

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gerlach KD, Spreng RN, Gilmore AW, Schacter DL (2011) Solving future problems: Default network and executive activity associated with goal-directed mental simulations. NeuroImage 55(4):1816–1824

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goulden N, Khusnulina A, Davis N, Bracewell RM, Bokde AL, McNulty JP, Mullins PG (2014) The salience network is responsible for switching between the default mode network and the central executive network: replication from DCM. NeuroImage 99:180–190

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gray JR, Chabris CF, Braver TS (2003) Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nat Neurosci 6(3):316–322

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greicius MD, Menon V (2004) Default–mode activity during a passive sensory task: uncoupled from deactivation but impacting activation. J Cogn Neurosci 16(9):1484–1492

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gusnard DA, Raichle ME (2001) Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2(10):685–694

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Honan CA, McDonald S, Gowland A, Fisher A, Randall RK (2015) Deficits in comprehension of speech acts after TBI: the role of theory of mind and executive function. Brain Lang 150(1):69–79

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hyatt CJ, Calhoun VD, Pearlson GD, Assaf M (2015) Specific default mode subnetworks support mentalizing as revealed through opposing network recruitment by social and semantic fMRI tasks. Hum Brain Mapp 36(8):3047–3063

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kane MJ, Brown LH, McVay JC, Silvia PJ, Myin-Germeys I, Kwapil TR (2007) For whom the mind wanders, and when. Psychol Sci 18(7):614–621

    Google Scholar 

  • Keenan T, Olson DR, Marini Z (1998) Working memory and children’s developing understanding of mind. Aust J Psychol 50(1):76–82

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kercood S, Grskovic JA, Banda D, Begeske J (2014) Working memory and autism: a review of literature. Res Autism Spect Dis 8(10):1316–1332

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killingsworth MA, Gilbert DT (2010) A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science 330(6006):932

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koban L, Pourtois G (2014) Brain systems underlying the affective and social monitoring of actions: an integrated review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 46:71–84

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lett TA, Voineskos AN, Kennedy JL, Levine B, Daskalakis ZJ (2014) Treating working memory deficits in schizophrenia: a review of the neurobiology. Biol Psychiatry 75(5):361–370

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lin S, Keysar B, Epley N (2010) Reflexively mindblind: using theory of mind to interpret behavior requires effortful attention. J Exp Soc Psychol 46(3):551–556

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linden DE (2007) The working memory networks of the human brain. Neuroscientist 13(3):257–267

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LoPresti ML, Schon K, Tricarico MD, Swisher JD, Celone KA, Stern CE (2008) Working memory for social cues recruits orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of delayed matching to sample for emotional expressions. J Neurosci 28(14):3718–3728

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Maehara Y (2014) Mind over–reading: Psychological research on working memory in theory of mind. Kyoto University Press, Kyoto (in Japanese)

    Google Scholar 

  • Maehara Y, Saito S (2011) I see into your mind too well: working memory adjusts the probability judgment of others’ mental states. Acta Psychol 138(3):367–376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maehara Y, Saito S (2013) Cognitive load on working memory both encourages and discourages reasoning bias regarding the mental states of others. Aust J Psychol 65(3):163–171

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mahy CEV, Moses LJ, Pfeifer JH (2014) How and where: theory–of–mind in the brain. Dev Cogn Neurosci 9(1):68–81

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mars RB, Neubert FX, Noonan MP, Sallet J, Toni I, Rushworth MF (2012) On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain”. Front Hum Neurosci 6:189

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mason MF, Norton MI, Van Horn JD, Wegner DM, Grafton ST, Macrae CN (2007) Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315(5810):393–395

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McKiernan KA, Kaufman JN, Kucera-Thompson J, Binder JR (2003) A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. J Cogn Neurosci 15(3):394–408

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McKinnon MC, Moscovitch M (2007) Domain-general contributions to social reasoning: theory of mind and deontic reasoning re-explored. Cognition 102(2):179–218

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McVay JC, Kane MJ (2012) Why does working memory capacity predict variation in reading comprehension? On the influence of mind wandering and executive attention. J Exp Psychol Gen 141(2):302–320

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer ML, Spunt RP, Berkman ET, Taylor SE, Lieberman MD (2012) Evidence for social working memory from a parametric functional MRI study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109(6):1883–1888

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake A, Shah P (eds) (1999) Models of working memory: mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Moses LJ (2001) Executive accounts of theory-of-mind development. Child Dev 72(3):688–690

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mulders PC, van Eijndhoven PF, Schene AH, Beckmann CF, Tendolkar I (2015) Resting-state functional connectivity in major depressive disorder: a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 56:330–344

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ouimet AJ, Gawronski B, Dozois DJA (2009) Cognitive vulnerability to anxiety: a review and an integrative model. Clin Psychol Rev 29(6):459–470

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E (2005) N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp 25(1):46–59

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perner J (1991) Understanding the representational mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL (2001) A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98(2):676–682

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Reniers RL, Corcoran R, Vollm BA, Mashru A, Howard R, Liddle PF (2012) Moral decision-making, ToM, empathy and the default mode network. Biol Psychol 90(3):202–210

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rypma B, Prabhakaran V, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD (1999) Load-dependent roles of frontal brain regions in the maintenance of working memory. NeuroImage 9(2):216–226

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe R, Carey S, Kanwisher N (2004) Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional imaging. Annu Rev Psychol 55:87–124

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schilbach L, Eickhoff SB, Rotarska-Jagiela A, Fink GR, Vogeley K (2008) Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the “default system” of the brain. Conscious Cogn 17(2):457–467

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schurz M, Radua J, Aichhorn M, Richlan F, Perner J (2014) Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 42:9–34

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, Kenna H, Reiss AL, Greicius MD (2007) Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci 27(9):2349–2356

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sheffield JM, Barch DM (2016) Cognition and resting-state functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 61:108–120

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidlauskaite J, Wiersema JR, Roeyers H, Krebs RM, Vassena E, Fias W, Brass B, Achten E, Sonuga-Barke E (2014) Anticipatory processes in brain state switching: Evidence from a novel cued-switching task implicating default mode and salience networks. NeuroImage 98:359–365

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood J (2011) Mind-wandering while reading: attentional decoupling, mindless reading and the cascade model of inattention. Lang Ling Compass 5(2):63–77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood J, Schooler JW (2015) The science of mind wandering: empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annu Rev Psychol 66:487–518

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood J, Brown K, Baird B, Schooler JW (2011) Cooperation between the default mode network and the frontal-parietal network in the production of an internal train of thought. Brain Res 1428:60–70

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith EE, Jonides J (1997) Working memory: a view from neuroimaging. Cogn Psychol 33(1):5–42

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spreng RN, Grady C (2010) Patterns of brain activity supporting autobiographical memory, prospection and theory-of-mind and their relationship to the default mode network. J Cogn Neurosci 22(6):1112–1123

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spreng R, Stevens WD, Chamberlain JP, Gilmore AW, Schacter DL (2010) Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. NeuroImage 53(1):303–317

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Menon V (2008) A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105(34):12569–12574

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Stone VE, Baron-Cohen S, Knight RT (1998) Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. J Cogn Neurosci 10(5):640–656

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Summerfield JJ, Hassabis D, Maguire EA (2010) Differential engagement of brain regions within a “core” network during scene construction. Neuropsychologia 48(5):1501–1509

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Towse JN, Towse AS, Saito S, Maehara Y, Miyake A (2016) Joint cognition: thought contagion and the consequences of cooperation when sharing the task of random sequence generation. PLoS One 11(3):e0151306

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Uddin LQ (2015) Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci 16(1):55–61

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uddin LQ, Supekar K, Menon V (2013) Reconceptualizing functional brain connectivity in autism from a developmental perspective. Front Hum Neurosci 7:458

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Unsworth N, McMillan BD (2013) Mind wandering and reading comprehension: examining the roles of working memory capacity, interest, motivation, and topic experience. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 39(3):832–842

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Unsworth N, Spillers GJ (2010) Working memory capacity: attention control, secondary memory, or both? A direct test of the dual-component model. J Mem Lang 62(4):392–406

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Overwalle F (2009) Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 30(3):829–858

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vogel EK, Machizawa MG (2004) Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity. Nature 428(6984):748–751

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wager TD, Smith EE (2003) Neuroimaging studies of working memory: a meta-analysis. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 3(4):255–274

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner DD, Haxby JV, Heatherton TF (2012) The representation of self and person knowledge in the medial prefrontal cortex. WIREs Cogn Sci 3(4):451–470

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman HM, Cross D, Watson J (2001) Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Dev 72(3):655–684

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Ford JM (2012) Default mode network activity and connectivity in psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 8:49–76

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer H, Perner J (1983) Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 13(1):103–128

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, Roffman JL, Smoller JW, Zöllei L, Polimeni JR, Fischl B, Liu H, Buckner RL (2011) The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol 106(3):1125–1165

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan RA, Radvansky GA (1998) Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychol Bull 123(2):162–185

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yukio Maehara .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer Japan KK

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Maehara, Y. (2017). Psychological and Neural Functions of Working Memory in Social Contexts. In: Tsukiura, T., Umeda, S. (eds) Memory in a Social Context. Springer, Tokyo. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-56591-8_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics