Skip to main content

Neural Correlates of Reasoning by Exclusion

  • Conference paper
Systems Neuroscience and Rehabilitation
  • 800 Accesses

Abstract

When asked to select a label for a novel object from a given group of labels that includes both novel and familiar labels, one tends to choose a novel label. Nonhuman animals robustly fail to demonstrate the same tendency, although this tendency called “exclusion” that can bias human behavior may seem quite natural. The functional magnetic resonance imaging study described here investigated the neural correlates of this bias. The subjects were trained on two sample-to-comparison associations. In the scanner, they were shown a novel sample and were asked to choose between a trained comparison and a novel comparison. The subjects readily chose the novel comparison and rejected the trained one, thus demonstrating exclusion. Significant activation was observed in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and inferior parietal lobule (IPL) during exclusion. Medial frontal activation was also observed when the novel stimuli appeared. These results suggest that the medial frontal cortex is associated with novelty detection and that the PFC and IPL are involved in rejecting the defined comparison.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    S− condition was not used in the analysis (Appendix 2)

References

  1. Aust U, Range F, Steurer M, Huber L (2008) Inferential reasoning by exclusion in pigeons, dogs, and humans. Anim Cogn 11:587–597

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Beran MJ, Washburn DA (2002) Chimpanzee responding during matching to sample: control by exclusion. J Exp Anal Behav 78:497–508

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Clement TS, Zentall TR (2003) Choice based on exclusion in pigeons. Psychon Bull Rev 10:959–964

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Markman EM, Wachtel GF (1988) Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cogn Psychol 20:121–157

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Markman EM, Wasow JL, Hansen MB (2003) Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners. Cogn Psychol 47:241–275

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Piccin TB, Blewitt P (2007) Resource conservation as a basis for the mutual exclusivity effect in children’s word learning. First Lang 27:5–28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. McIlvane WJ, Kledaras JB, Munson LC, King KAJ, Derose JC, Stoddard LT (1987) Controlling relations in conditional discrimination and matching by exclusion. J Exp Anal Behav 48:187–208

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Stromer R (1989) Symmetry of control by exclusion in humans’ arbitrary matching to sample. Psychol Rep 64:915–922

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T, Fujita K, Yamamoto J (1991) Emergence of symmetry in a visual conditional discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Psychol Rep 68:51–60

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Tomonaga M (1993) Tests for control by exclusion and negative stimulus relations of arbitrary matching to sample in a symmetry-emergent chimpanzee. J Exp Anal Behav 59:215–229

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Kastak CR, Schusterman RJ (2002) Sea lions and equivalence: expanding classes by exclusion. J Exp Anal Behav 78:449–465

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Dugdale N, Lowe CF (2000) Testing for symmetry in the conditional discriminations of language-trained chimpanzees. J Exp Anal Behav 73:5–22

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Ogawa A, Yamazaki Y, Ueno K, Cheng K, Iriki A (2010) Inferential reasoning by exclusion recruits parietal and prefrontal cortices. Neuroimage 52:1603–1610

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Dickins DW, Singh KD, Roberts N, Burns P, Downes JJ, Jimmieson P, Bentall RP (2001) An fMRI study of stimulus equivalence. Neuroreport 12:405–411

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Schlund MW, Hoehn-Saric R, Cataldo MF (2007) New knowledge derived from learned knowledge: functional–anatomic correlates of stimulus equivalence. J Exp Anal Behav 87:287–307

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Schlund MW, Cataldo MF, Hoehn-Saric R (2008) Neural correlates of derived relational responding on tests of stimulus equivalence. Behav Brain Funct 4:6

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Ogawa A, Yamazaki Y, Ueno K, Cheng K, Iriki A (2010) Neural correlates of species-typical illogical cognitive bias in human inference. J Cogn Neurosci 22:2120–2130

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Dickins DW (2005) On aims and methods in the neuroimaging of derived relations. J Exp Anal Behav 84:453–483

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Cowley BJ, Green G, Braunling-McMorrow D (1992) Using stimulus equivalence procedures to teach name-face matching to adults with brain injuries. J Appl Behav Anal 25:461–475

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Guercio JM, Podolska-Schroeder H, Rehfeldt RA (2004) Using stimulus equivalence technology to teach emotion recognition to adults with acquired brain injury. Brain Injury 18:593–601

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Gold M, Adair JC, Jacobs DH, Heilman KM (1995) Right-left confusion in Gerstmann’s syndrome – a mode of body-centered spatial orientation. Cortex 31:267–283

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Horwitz B, Rumsey JM, Donohue BC (1998) Functional connectivity of the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:8939–8944

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Göbel S, Walsh V, Rushworth M (2001) The mental number line and the human angular gyrus. Neuroimage 14:1278–1289

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Cattaneo Z, Silvanto J, Pascual-Leone A, Battelli L (2009) The role of the angular gyrus in the modulation of visuospatial attention by the mental number line. Neuroimage 44:563–568

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Samson D, Apperly IA, Chiavarino C, Humphreys GW (2004) Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else’s belief. Nat Neurosci 7:499–500

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Aichhorn M, Perner J, Kronbichler M, Staffen W, Ladurner G (2006) Do visual perspective tasks need theory of mind? Neuroimage 30:1059–1068

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Perner J, Aichhorn M, Kronbichler M, Staffen W, Ladurner G (2006) Thinking of mental and other representations: the roles of left and right temporo-parietal junction. Soc Neurosci 1:245–258

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Husain M, Nachev P (2007) Space and the parietal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 11:30–36

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Akitoshi Ogawa .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Appendices

Appendix 1: Image Acquisition and Analysis

The brain images were collected using a 4 T Varian Unity Inova MRI system. The BOLD signal was measured using a T2*-weighted echo planar imaging sequence (TR = 2,600 ms, TE = 25 ms, FA = 40°). Twenty-five axial slices (thickness = 5.0 mm, gap = 0 mm, FOV = 240 × 240 mm, matrix = 64 × 64) were acquired per volume. A set of high-resolution T1-weighted structural images was obtained by magnetization-prepared 3D FLASH (TR = 110 ms, TE = 6.2 ms, FA = 11°, matrix = 256 × 256 × 180, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm3).

The functional and structural images were analyzed with Brain Voyager QX. The functional images for each subject were preprocessed, including slice time correction, three-dimensional motion correction, spatial smoothing with a Gaussian filter (FWHM = 6 mm) and high-pass filtering (0.01 Hz). The structural image was transformed into the standard Talairach space. The functional images were transformed into the standard Talairach space by normalizing and resizing them to the transformed structural image. BOLD signals were modeled using a synthetic hemodynamic response function composed of two gamma functions. Random-effects analysis was performed on the functional data to reveal significant activation.

Appendix 2: S− Condition

In S−, a novel comparison replaced the relevant trained comparison, whereas in S+, a novel comparison replaced the trained irrelevant comparison. The S− condition was not used in the analysis because eight subjects did not respond as expected to the S− condition. Four of the subjects chose the defined comparison (C2 was chosen in the presence of S1), whereas the other four subjects responded randomly. To respond accurately, it was necessary to reject the defined comparison and choose the novel one, based on the negative relations of ‘if S1, then not C2’ and ‘if S2, then not C1’. There are two possible explanations for this failure: one is that the training was insufficient, and the other is that the subjects could not reject the defined comparison stimulus. The latter possibility is not plausible, because the subjects rejected the defined comparison stimulus in Exclusion. These results suggest that the subjects did not learn the negative relations sufficiently.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer

About this paper

Cite this paper

Ogawa, A. (2011). Neural Correlates of Reasoning by Exclusion. In: Kansaku, K., Cohen, L.G. (eds) Systems Neuroscience and Rehabilitation. Springer, Tokyo. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-54008-3_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-54008-3_9

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Tokyo

  • Print ISBN: 978-4-431-53998-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-4-431-54008-3

  • eBook Packages: MedicineMedicine (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics