Skip to main content

Kognitive Entwicklung

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Entwicklungspsychologie des Jugendalters

Part of the book series: Springer-Lehrbuch ((SLB))

  • 64k Accesses

Zusammenfassung

Das Kapitel bietet einen Überblick zu zentralen Aspekten der kognitiven Entwicklung im Jugendalter. Als wichtige Bestandteile der allgemeinen Informationsverarbeitung werden zunächst die Konstrukte Intelligenz und Arbeitsgedächtnis vorgestellt und diesbezügliche Besonderheiten im Jugendalter geschildert. Zur Beschreibung komplexerer kognitiver Prozesse werden zudem beispielhaft Erkenntnisse aus der Forschung zu sozialen Kognitionen und zur Selbstregulation näher beschrieben und deren Bedeutung für Entwicklungsprozesse im Jugendalter hervorgehoben. Abschließend werden mit den schulischen Leistungen und der Nutzung digitaler Medien ausgewählte Bereiche beleuchtet, die in deutlichem Zusammenhang zur kognitiven Entwicklung und der daraus resultierenden kognitiven Leistungsfähigkeit stehen.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 34.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 44.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Literatur

  • Alloway, T. P., & Alloway, R. G. (2013). Working memory across the lifespan: A cross-sectional approach. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 84–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (2006). Verbal and visuo-spatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Development, 77, 1698–1716.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., Rothstein, H. R., & Saleem, M. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behaviour in Eastern and Western countries: A meta–analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 151–171.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Appel, M., & Schreiner, C. (2014). Digitale Demenz? Mythen und wissenschaftliche Befundlage zur Auswirkung von Internetnutzung. Psychologische Rundschau., 65, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829–839.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Hrsg.), Recent advances in learning and motivation (Vol. 8, S. 47–90). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beier, M. E., & Ackerman, P. L. (2004). A reappraisal of the relationship between span memory and intelligence via „best evidence synthesis“. Intelligence, 32, 607–619.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78, 246–263.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, J. N., Leary, S. D., Joinson, C., Ness, A. R., Tomporowski, P. D., Boyle, J. M., & Reilly, J. J. (2013). Associations between objectively measured physical activity and academic attainment in adolescents from a UK cohort. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 48, 265–270.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. New York. NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. (2005). The three-stratum theory of cognitive abilities. In D. P. Flanagan & P. L. Harrison (Hrsg.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (2 Aufl., S. 69–76). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1992). The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B. (1987). Intelligence: Its structure, growth, and action. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiu, Y., Kao, C., & Reynolds, B. L. (2012). The relative effectiveness of digital game-based learning types in English as a foreign language setting: A meta-analysis. British Journal of Educational Technology, 43, 104–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colom, R., Flores-Mendoza, C., Quiroga, M. A., & Privado, J. (2005). Working memory and general intelligence: The role of short-term storage. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 1005–1014.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., Bunting, M. F., Therriault, D. J., & Minkoff, S. R. B. (2002). A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 30, 163–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deary, I. J., Whiteman, M. C., Starr, J. M., Whalley, L. J., & Fox, H. C. (2004). The impact of childhood intelligence on later life: Following up the Scottish mental surveys of 1932 and 1947. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 130–147.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K. A. (1986). A social information processing model of social competence in children. In M. Perlmutter (Hrsg.), Cognitive perspectives on children’s social and behavioral development Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology, (Bd. 18, S. 77–125). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K. A., Dishion, T. J., & Lansford, J. E. (Hrsg.). (2006). Deviant peer influences in programs for youth: Problems and solutions. New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duckworth, A. L., Kirby, T. A., Gollwitzer, A., & Oettingen, G. (2013). From fantasy to action mental contrasting with implementation intentions (MCII) improves academic performance in children. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 745–753.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. (2005). Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychological Science, 16, 939–944.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fink, A., Luttenberger, S., Krammer, A., Macher, D., & Papousek, I. (2015). Die Veränderung kognitiver Fähigkeiten über die Sommerferien. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 62, 303–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fry, A. F., & Hale, S. (1996). Processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence: Evidence for a developmental cascade. Psychological Science, 7, 237–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177–190.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (2005). The origin of mind: Evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, A., Oettingen, G., Kirby, T. A., Duckworth, A. L., & Mayer, D. (2011). Mental contrasting facilitates academic performance in school children. Motivation and Emotion, 35, 403–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottschling, J., Spengler, M., Spinath, B., & Spinath, F. M. (2012). The prediction of school achievement from a behavior genetic perspective: Results from the German twin study on cognitive ability, self-reported motivation, and school achievement (CoSMoS). Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 381–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J. L., & McArdle, J. J. (2007). Understanding human intelligence since Spearman. In R. Cudeck & R. C. MacCallum (Hrsg.), Factor analysis at 100: Historical developments and future directions (S. 205–247). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Westport: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, C. N., & Wellman, H. M. (1980). Children’s developing understanding of mental verbs: Remember, know, and guess. Child Development, 51, 1095–1102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawada, C. L. K. (2004). Self-regulatory thought in goal setting: Perceptual and cognitive processes. Dissertation Abstracts International, 64, 12B. (UMI No. 0419-4217).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kray, J., & Lindenberger, U. (2007). Fluide Intelligenz. In J. Brandstädter & U. Lindenberger (Hrsg.), Entwicklungspsychologie der Lebensspanne (S.194–220). Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunert, H. J., Derichs, G., & Irle, E. (1996). Entwicklung von Aufmerksamkeitsfunktionen im Kindesalter: Ergebnisse einer vorläufigen Normierung der computergestützten Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung (TAP) an 9- bis 12jährigen Kindern. Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, 7, 92–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maass, A., Klöpper, K. M., Michel, F., & Lohaus, A. (2011). Does media use have a short-term impact on cognitive performance? A study on television viewing and video gaming. Journal of Media Psychology, 23, 65–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mägi, K., Männamaa, M., & Kikas, E. (2016). Profiles of self-regulation in elementary grades: Relations to math and reading skills. Learning and Individual Differences, 51, 37–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Means, B., Toyama, Y., Murphy, R., Bakia, M., & Jones, K. (2010). Evaluation of evidence-based practices in online learning: A meta-analysis and review of online learning studies. Washington: U.S. Department of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meisenberg, G. (2010). The reproduction of intelligence. Intelligence, 38, 220–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mills, K. L., Dumontheil, I., Speekenbrink, M., & Blakemore, S. J. (2015). Multitasking during social interactions in adolescence and early adulthood. Open Science, 2, 150–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine children’s motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 33–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neubauer, A. C. (1995). Intelligenz und Geschwindigkeit der Informationsverarbeitung. Wien: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oberauer, K., Sü, H.-M., Wilhelm, O., & Wittmann, W. W. (2008). Which working memory functions predict intelligence? Intelligence, 36, 641–652.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oettingen, G., & Kluge, L. (2009). Kluges Zielsetzen durch mentales Kontrastieren von Zukunft und Realität. In T. Iwers-Stelljes (Hrsg.), Prävention – Intervention – Konfliktlösung (S. 215–227). Wiesbaden: VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oettingen, G., Mayer, D., & Thorpe, J. (2010). Self-regulation of commitment to reduce cigarette consumption: Mental contrasting of future with reality. Psychology and Health, 25, 961–977.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parks-Stamm, E. J., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2010). Implementation intentions and test anxiety: Shielding academic performance from distraction. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 30–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paus, T. (2005). Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 60–68.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prensky, M. (2001). Digital game based learning. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramsden, S., Richardson, F. M., Josse, G., Thomas, M. S., Ellis, C., Shakeshaft, C., ... & Price, C. J. (2011). Verbal and non-verbal intelligence changes in the teenage brain. Nature, 479(7371), 113–116.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rindermann, H. (2011). Intelligenzwachstum in Kindheit und Jugend. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 58, 210–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, B., Becker, N., Romeyke, S., Schäfer, S., Domnick, F., & Spinath, F. M. (2015). Intelligence and school grades: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 53, 118–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rost, D. H. (2013). Handbuch Intelligenz. Weinheim: Beltz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selman, R. L. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding: Developmental and clinical analyses. New York, NY: Academic Press (dt. 1984, Die Entwicklung des sozialen Verstehens. Entwicklungspsychologische und klinische Untersuchungen. Frankfurt a. M.: Suhrkamp).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Lockl, K. (2006). Entwicklung metakognitiver Kompetenzen im Kindes- und Jugendalter. In W. Schneider & B. Sodian (Hrsg.), Kognitive Entwicklung – Enzyklopädie der Psychologie. Entwicklungspsychologie (Bd. 2, S. 721–767). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1997). Memory development between 2 and 20. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sparrow, B., Liu, J., & Wegner, D. M. (2011). Google effects on memory: Cognitive consequences of having information at our fingertips. Science, 333(6043), 776–778.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spearman, C. E. (1927). The abilities of man, their nature and measurement. New York, NY: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stadler, G., Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2010). Intervention effects of information and self-regulation on eating fruits and vegetables over two years. Health Psychology, 29, 274–283.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, E. & Grabner, R. (2014): Die Erforschung menschlicher Intelligenz. In Ahnert, L. (Hrsg.): Theorien der Entwicklungspsychologie (S. 174–197). Heidelberg: Spektrum

    Google Scholar 

  • Storm, B. C., Stone, S. M., & Benjamin, A. S. (2016). Using the Internet to access information inflates future use of the Internet to access other information. Memory, 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Süss, H. M., Oberauer, K., Wittmann, W. W., Wilhelm, O., & Schulze, R. (2002). Working-memory capacity explains reasoning ability – and a little bit more. Intelligence, 30, 261–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary mental abilities. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, J. J., Vogel, D. S., Cannon-Bowers, J., Bowers, C. A., Muse, K., & Wright, M. (2006). Computer gaming and interactive simulations for learning: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 34, 229–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wach, F. S., Spengler, M., Gottschling, J., & Spinath, F. M. (2015). Sex differences in secondary school achievement–The contribution of self-perceived abilities and fear of failure. Learning and Instruction, 36, 104–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1979). A theory of motivation for some classroom experiences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 3–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 548–573.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeager, D. S., Miu, A. S., Powers, J., & Dweck, C. S. (2013). Implicit theories of personality and attributions of hostile intent: A meta-analysis, an experiment, and a longitudinal intervention. Child Development, 84, 1651–1667.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeates, K. O., & Selman, R. L. (1989). Social competence in the schools: Toward an integrative developmental model for intervention. Developmental Review, 9, 64–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining Self-Regulation: A social cognitive perspective. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Hrsg.), Handbook of Self-Regulation (S. 13–39). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sven Lindberg .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer-Verlag GmbH Deutschland

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Lindberg, S., Hasselhorn, M. (2018). Kognitive Entwicklung. In: Lohaus, A. (eds) Entwicklungspsychologie des Jugendalters. Springer-Lehrbuch. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-55792-1_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-55792-1_3

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-662-55791-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-662-55792-1

  • eBook Packages: Psychology (German Language)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics