Skip to main content

The Feasible Infeasibility of Activity Scheduling

  • Conference paper
Human Behaviour and Traffic Networks

Abstract

This chapter attempts to specify realistic behavioral assumptions in models of linked choices of activity sequence/duration, destination, mode, departure time, and route. It is argued that linked choices or activity scheduling may be feasible because people employ heuristic decision rules that circumvent their limits on information-processing capacity. In addition, learning is argued to play an important role. The outcome of learning is either that aggregated information about linked choices (scripts) is retrieved from memory and executed without deliberation, or that deliberate choices are simplified because they are made among sets of scripts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aarts, H., and Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). The automatic activation of goal—directed behaviour: The case of travel habit. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 20, 75–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abelson, R. P. (1981). Psychological status of the script concept. American Psychologist, 36, 715–729.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arentze, T., and Timmermans, H. P. J. (2000). ALBATROSS: A learning based transportation oriented simulation system. Eindhoven, The Netherlands: Technical University of Eindhoven, European Institute of Retailing and Services Studies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arentze, T., and Timmermans, H. P. J. (2002). Modeling learning and adaptation processes in ctivity-travel choice: A framework and numerical experiments. Transportation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arkes, H. R., and Ayton, P. (1999). The sunk cost and Concorde effects: Are humans less rational than lower animals? Psychological Bulletin, 125, 591–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arkes, H. R., and Blumer, C. (1985). The psychology of sunk costs. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 35, 124–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Axhausen, K., and Gärling, T. (1992). Activity-based approaches to travel analysis: Conceptual frameworks, models, and research problems. Transport Reviews, 12, 323–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (1997). Human memory: Theory and practice (rev. ed.). Hove, UK: Psychology Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslaysky, E., Muraven, M., and Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Akiva, M., and Bowman, J. L. (1997). Activity based disaggregate travel demand model system with daily activity schedules. Transportation Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Akiva, M., and Lerman, S. (1985). Discrete choice analysis: Theory and applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bhat, C. R., and Koppelman, F. S. (1993). A conceptual framework of individual activity program generation. Transportation Research A, 27, 447–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bhat, C. R., and Koppelman, F. S. (2000). Activity-based travel demand analysis: History, results and future directions. Transportation Research Record.

    Google Scholar 

  • Björkman, M. (1984). Decision making, risk taking, and psychological time: Review of empirical findings and psychological theory. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 25, 31–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boe, O., and Gärling, T. (2002). Integration of concurrent decisions. Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. London: Pergamon. Crowder, R. (1982). Principles of learning and memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doherty, S. T., Axhausen, K., Gärling, T., and Miller, E. J. (2002). A conceptual model of the weekly household activity-travel scheduling process. In E. Stern, I. Salomon and P. Bovy (Eds.), Travel behaviour: Patterns, Implications and modelling. Glos, UK: Edward Elgar. In press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., and Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Forth Worth: Harcourt Brace. Ericsson, A., and Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence of maximal adaptation to task constrints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ettema, D. (1996). Activity-based travel demand modeling. Doctoral dissertation, Department of Architecture, Technical University of Eindhoven, The Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ettema, D., and Timmermans, H. P. J. (1997). Theories and models of activity patterns. In D. Ettema and H. J. P. Timmermans (Eds.), Activity-based approaches to travel analysis (pp. 1–36 ). Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: Th emode model as an integrative framework. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 23, 75–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, S. L, and Scholnick, E. K. (Eds.) (1997), The developmental psychology of planning. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Boe, O., and Golledge, R. G. (2000). Determinants of distance thresholds for driving. Transportation Research Record No. 1718, 68–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Brännäs, K., Garvill, J., Golledge, R. G., Gopal, S., Holm, E., and Lindberg, E. (1989). Household activity scheduling. In Transport policy, management and technology towards 2001 (Vol. IV, pp 235–248). Ventura, CA: Western Periodicals.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Eek, D., Loukopoulos, P., Fujii, S., Johansson-Stenman, O., Kitamura, R., Pendyala, R., and Vilhelmson, B. (2002). A conceptual analysis of the impact of travel demand management on private car use. Transport Policy. In press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Fujii, S., and Boe, O. (2001). Empirical tests of a model of determinants of script-based driving choice. Transportation Research F, 4, 89–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Gillholm, R., Romanus, J., and Selart, M. (1997). Interdependent activity and travel choices: Behavioral principles of integration of choice outcomes. In D. F. Et-tema and H. P. J. Timmermans (Eds.), Activity-based approaches to travel analysis (pp. 135–150 ). Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., and Golledge, R. G. (1989). Environmental perception and cognition. In E. H. Zube and G. T. Moore (Eds.), Advances in environment, behavior, and design (Vol. 2, pp. 203–236 ). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., and Golledge, R. G. (2000). Cognitive mapping and spatial decision making. In R. Kitchin and S. Freundschuh (Eds.), Cognitive mapping: Past, present, and future (pp. 44–65 ). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Kalén, T., Romanus, J., Selart, M., and Vilhelmson, B. (1998). Computer simulation of a theory of household activity scheduling. Environment and Planning A, 30, 665–679.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Karlsson, N., Romanus, J., and Selart, M. (1997). Influences of the past on choices of the future. In R. Ranyard, R. Crozier and O. Svenson (Eds.), Decision making: Cognitive models and explanations (pp. 167–188 ). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Kwan, M.-P., and Golledge, R. G. (1994). Computational-process modeling of household activity scheduling. Transportation Research B, 25, 355–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., and Romanus, J. (1997). Integration and segregation of prior outcomes in risky decisions. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 38, 289–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., and Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 98, 506–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R. G. (Ed.), Wayfinding: Cognitive maps and spatial behavior. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golob, T. F. (1989). The causal influences of income and car ownership on trip generation by mode. Journal of Transport Economics and Policy, 23, 141–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golob, T. F., and McNally, M. G. (1997). A model of household interactions in activity participation and the derived demand for travel. Transportation Research B, 31, 177–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hägerstrand, T. (1970). What about people in regional science? Papers of the Regional Science Association, 24, 7–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, S., and Schwab, M. (1995). Describing disaggregate flows: Individual and household activity patterns. In S. Hanson (Ed.), The geography of urban transportation (pp. 166–187 ). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, S. A., and Hastie, R. (1990). Hindsight: Biased judgments of past events after the outcomes are known. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 311–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes-Roth, B., and Hayes-Roth, F. (1979). A cognitive model of planning. Cognitive Science, 3, 275–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, P., Dix, M. C., Clarke, M. I., and Heggie, I. G. (1983). Understanding travel behavior. Aldershot, UK: Gower.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrika, 47, 263–291.

    Article  MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Kimble, G. A. (1992). Conditioning and learning. In S. Koch and D. E. Leary (Eds.), A century of psychology as science (pp. 284–321 ). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitamura, R. (1984). A model of daily time allocation to discretionary out-of-home activities and trips. Transportation Research B, 18, 255–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitamura, R. (1988). An evaluation of activity-based travel analysis. Transportation, 15, 934.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitamura, R., and Fujii, S. (1998). Two computational process models of activity-travel choice. In T. Gärling, T. Laitila and K. Westin (Eds.), Theoretical foundations of travel choice modeling (pp. 251–279 ). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitamura, R., Fujii, S., and Pas, E. I. (1997). Time-use data, analysis and modeling: toward the next generation of transportation planning methodologies. Transport Policy, 4, 225–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, G. (1997). The recognition-primed decision (RPD) model: Looking back, looking forward. In C. E. Zsambok and G. Klein (Eds.), Naturalistic decision making (pp. 285292 ). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lachman, R., Lachman, J. L., and Buterfield, E. C. (1979). Cognitive psychology and information processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larrick, R. P. (1993). Motivational factors in decision theories: The role of self-protection. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 440–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 272–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mahmassani, H. S., and Jou, R.-C. (2000). Transferring insights into commuter behavior dynamics from laboratory experiments to field surveys. Transportation Research A, 34, 243–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • McFadden, D. (2001). Disaggregate behavioral travel demand’s RUM side: A 30-year retrospective. In D. Hensher (Ed.), Travel behavior research: The leading edge (pp. 1763 ). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., and Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oliver, R. L. (1997). Satisfaction: A behavioral perspective on the consumer. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortúzar, J. de D., and Willumsen, L. G. (1994). Modelling transport ( 2nd ed. ). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pas, E. I. (1998). Time in travel choice modeling: From relative obscurity to center stage. In T. Gärling, T. Laitila, and K. Westin (Eds.) Theoretical foundations of travel choice modeling (pp. 231–250 ). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., and Johnson, E. J. (1993). The adaptive decision maker. New York: Cambridge University Press

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Recker, W. W., and McNally, M. G. (1986). An activity-based modeling framework for transportation policy evaluation. In Behavioral research for transport (pp. 31–52 ). Utrecht, The Netherlands: VNU Science Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Recker, W. W., McNally, M. G., and Root, G. S. (1986). A model of complex travel behavior. Part 1: Theoretical development. Transportation Research A, 20, 319–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ronis, D. L., Yates, J. F., and Kirscht, J. P. (1989). Attitudes, decisions, and habits as determinants of repeated behavior. In A. R. Pratkanis, S. J. Breckler and A. G. Greenwald (eds.) Attitude structure and function (pp. 213–239 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Root, G. S., and Recker, W. W. (1983). Towards a dynamic model of individual activity pattern formulation. In S. Carpenter and P. Jones (Eds.), Recent advances in travel demand analysis (pp. 371–382 ). Aldershot, UK: Gower.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1982). Models of bounded rationality. Vol 2: Behavioral economics and business organization. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1990). Invariants of human behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staw, B. M. (1997). The escalation of commitment: An update and appraisal. In Z. Shapira (Ed.), Organizational decision making (pp. 191–215 ). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Svenson, O. (1998). The perspective from behavioral decision theory on modeling travel choice. In T. Gärling, T. Laitila and K. Westin (Eds.), Theoretical foundations of travel choice modeling (pp. 141–172 ). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 67–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R. H. (1985). Mental accounting and consumer choice. Marketing Science, 4, 199–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Timmermans, H. J. P., and Golledge, R. G. (1990). Applications of behavioral research on spatial problems II: Preference and choice. Progress in Human Geography, 14, 311–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rat and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A. (1969). Intransitivity of preferences. Psychological Review, 76, 31–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A. (1972). Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice. Psychological Review, 79, 281–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–58.

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B., and Aarts, H. (1999). Habit, attitude and planned behavior: Is habit an empty construct or an interesting case of goal-directed automatic? European Review of Social Psychology, 10, 101–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B., Aarts, H., and van Knippenberg, A. (1997). Habit, information acquisition, and the process of making travel mode choices. European Journal of Social Psychology 27: 539–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B., Aarts, H., van Knippenberg, A., and van Knippenberg, C. (1994). Attitude versus general habit: Antecedents of travel mode choice. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 285–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winston, G. C. (1987). Activity choice: A new approach to economic behavior. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 8, 567–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vlek, C., and Michon, J. (1992). Why we should and how we could decrease the use of motor vehicles in the near future. IATSS Research 15, 82–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2004 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this paper

Cite this paper

Gärling, T. (2004). The Feasible Infeasibility of Activity Scheduling. In: Schreckenberg, M., Selten, R. (eds) Human Behaviour and Traffic Networks. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-07809-9_10

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-07809-9_10

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-05950-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-662-07809-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics