Skip to main content

Gene-Culture Coevolution: Culture and Biology in Darwinian Perspective

  • Chapter
The Alphabet and the Brain
  • 174 Accesses

Abstract

It has become clear that the idiosyncratic properties of the human mind have a powerful effect on the evolution of culture. In some reciprocal manner as yet less easily grasped, culture has influenced the genetic evolution of the brain structures underlying the mind. Because this coevolution concerns both the biological and social sciences, efforts to construct provisional models and unify the still fragmentary evidence to explain it are of more than ordinary interest. With respect to the genesis of our own species, evolutionary biologists have begun to grapple with the fact that human beings were not created by the purely Darwinian evolution so relevant to other species. For the past several million years our ancestors have been shaped by biological evolution and cultural evolution proceeding together, in a manner still little understood. In each of 200000 or so generations two tracks of heritable information, one genetic and the other cultural, have met in the events of individual socialization. During this time biology does not appear to have overwhelmed culture and culture has not granted biology total independence from human affairs. The relationship resembles one of reciprocating interaction, in which culture is generated and shaped by biological imperatives while the course of genetic evolution shifts in response to cultural innovations.

Research support was provided in part through the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC), grant number A0393, and the Medical Research Council of Canada (MRC), grant number MA-8635. The author is a career Scholar of the MRC

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Amoore, J. E. (1977). Specific anosmia and the concept of primary odors. Chemical Senses and Flavour, 2, 267 — 281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aoki, K. (1986). A stochastic model of gene-culture coevolution suggested by the “culture historical hypothesis” for the evolution of adult lactose absorption in man. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 83, 2929 — 2933.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashton, G. C., Polovina, J.J., and Vandenberg, S. G. (1979). Segregation analysis of family data for 15 tests of cognitive ability. Behavior Genetics, 9, 329— 347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, P. L., and Luckmann, T. (1966). The social construction of reality. A treatise in the sociology of knowledge. Garden City, New York: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlin, B., and Kay, P. (1969). Basic color terms: their universality and evolution. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berwick, R. C. (1982). Locality principles and the acquisition of syntactic knowledge. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science, MIT, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bliss, T. V. P., and Errington, M. L. (1977). “Reeler” mutant mice fail to show spontaneous alternation. Brain Research, 124, 168–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blumberg, B. S., and Hesser, J. E. (1975). Anthropology and infectious disease. In A. Damon (Ed.), Physiological anthropology. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 260–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, K. (1980). Human nature and history: a response to sociobiology. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H. (1973). Color vision and color naming: a psychophysiological hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 257–285.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H. (1975). Qualities of color vision in infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 19, 401–419.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H. (1979). Perceptual development: stability and change in feature perception. In M. H. Bornstein, and W. Kessen (Eds.), Psychological development from infancy: image to intention (pp. 37–81 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M.H. (1981). “Human infant color vision and color perception” reviewed and reassessed: a critique of Werner and Wooten (1979 a). Infant Behavior and Development, 4, 119–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M.H., and Marks, L. E. (1982). Color revisionism. Psychology Today, 16, 64–88, 68–70, 73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. W., Kessen, W., and Weiskopf, S. ( 1976 a). Color vision and hue categorization in young human infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology and Human Perception and Performance, 2, 115–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M.H., Kessen, W., and Weiskopf, S. (1976b). The categories of hue in infancy. Science, 191, 201–202.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T.J., Jr., and McGue, M. (1981). Familial studies of intelligence: a review. Science, 212, 1 055–1059.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brillouin, L. (1962). Science and information theory. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunswick, E. (1956). Perception and the representative design of experiments. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Budge, E. A. W. (1929). The Rosetta stone in the British Museum. London: The Religious Tract Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, A. L. (Ed.) (1978). The sociobiology debate: readings on ethical and scientific issues. New York: Harper and Rowe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caviness, V.S., Jr., and Rakic, P. (1978). Mechanisms of cortical development: a view from mutations in mice. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 1, 297–326.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chaitin, E. (1975). Randomness and mathematical proof. Scientific American, 232, 47–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiva, M. (1979). Comment la personne se construit en mangeant. Communications (École des Hautes Etudes en Sciences Sociales - Centre d’Études Transdisciplinaires, Paris ), 31, 107–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press. Daly, M., and Wilson, M. ( 1978 ). Sex, evolution, and behavior. North Scituate, MA: Duxburg Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dasen, P.R. (1972). Cross-cultural Piagetian research: a summary. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 3, 23–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeCasper, A.J., and Fifer, W.P. (1980). Of human bonding: newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science, 208, 1174–1176.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Durham, W. H. (1982 a). Interactions of genetic and cultural evolution: models and examples. Human Ecology, 10, 289–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durham, W.H. ( 1982 b). The coevolution of yam festivals and sickle-cell frequencies in West Africa. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 57, 183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1979). Human ethology: concepts and implications for the sciences of man. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2, 1–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eilers, R. E., Wilson, W.R., Moore, J. M. (1977). Developmental changes in speech discrimination in infants. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 20, 766–780.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eimas, P.D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P., and Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303–306.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fantz, R. L., Fagan III, J.F., and Miranda, S. B. (1975). Early visual selectivity: as a function of pattern variables, previous exposure, age from birth and conception, and expected cognitive deficit. In L. B. Cohen and P. Salapatek (Eds.), Infant perception: from sensation to cognition: basic visual processes (Vol. 1, pp. 249–345 ). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind: an essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, R., and McDaniel, C. (1982). The perception of biological motion by human infants. Science, 218, 486–487.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, D. G. (1974). Human infancy: an evolutionary perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, D. (1983). Margaret Mead and Samoa: the making and unmaking of an anthropological myth. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gajdusek, D.C. (1970). Physiological and psychological characteristics of stone age man. Science and Technology, 33, 26–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gatlin, L. L. (1972). Information theory and the living system. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gleason, H. A. (1961). An introduction to descriptive linguistics. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, S. (1953). Information theory. New York: Dover.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodenough, W. (1983). Margaret Mead and cultural anthropology. Science, 220, 906, 908. Hallpike, C. R. ( 1979 ). The foundations of primitive thought. New York: Oxford University (Clarendon) Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. (1968). The rise of anthropological theory: a history of theories of culture. New York: Harper and Rowe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. (1979). Cultural materialism: the struggle for a science of culture. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hershenson, M., Munsinger, H., and Kessen, W. (1965). Preference for shapes of intermediate variability in the newborn human. Science, 147, 630–631.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hockett, C. F., and Ascher, R. (1964). The human revolution. Current Anthropology, 5, 135–147, 166–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holloway, R. L. (1966). Cranial capacity, neural organization, and hominid evolution: a search for more suitable parameters. American Anthropologist, 68, 103–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horton, D. L., Mills, C. B. (1984). Human learning and memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 35, 361–394.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchinson, A. (1970). Labanotation or kinetography Laban: the system of analyzing and recording movement. New York: Theatre Arts Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irons, W. (1979). Cultural and biological success. In N.A. Chagnon and W. Irons (Eds.), Evolutionary biology and human social behavior: an anthropological perspective (pp 257–272 ). North Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E., Aoki, C. ( 1986, to be submitted). The gene-culture coevolution of alcohol use. Journal of Social and Biological Structures.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J. (1984). The nature of the child. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay, P., and McDaniel, C.K. (1978). The linguistic significance of the meanings of basic color terms. Language, 54, 610–646.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keesing, R. M. (1976). Cultural anthropology: a contemporary perspective. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1979). Semantic and cognitive development: an ontological perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1981). Constraints on knowledge and cognitive development. Physiological Reviews, 88, 197–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Khinchin, A. I. (1957). Mathematical foundations of information theory. (Silverman, R. A. and Friedman, M. D., Trans). New York: Dover.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klaus, M.H., Jerauld, R., Kreger, N. C., McAlpine, W., Steffa, M., and Kennel, J. H. (1972). Maternal attachment: importance of the first post-partum days. New England Journal of Medicine, 286, 460–463.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kroeber, A. L., and Kluckhohn, C. (1963). Culture: a critical review of concepts and definitions. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laban, R. (1975). Laban’s principles of dance and movement notation ( 2nd ed. ). London: MacDonald and Evans.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J., McDermott J., Simon, D.P., and Simon, H.A. (1980). Expert and novice performance in solving physics problems. Science, 208, 1335–1342.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lennie, P. (1984). Recent developments in the physiology of color vision. Trends in Neurosciences, 7, 243–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lenski, G., and Lenski, J. (1970). Human societies: a macrolevel introduction to sociology. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, W.J., Depper, J. M., Kanehisa, M., Kronke, M., Peffer, N.J., Svetlik, P.B., Sullivan, M., and Greene, W. C. (1985). Structure of the interleukin-2 receptor gene. Science, 230, 633–639.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewontin, R. C. (1981). Sleight of hand. The Sciences, 21, 23–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D.P., and Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431–461.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lisker, L., and Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: acoustical measurements. Word, 20, 384–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lockhard, J. S., Daley, P. C., Gunderson, V.M. (1979). Maternal and paternal differences in infant carry: U.S. and African data. American Naturalist, 113, 235–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loehlin, J. C., and Nichols, R. C. (1976). Heredity, environment, and personality. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden, C. J. (1983). Gene-culture coevolution and the devolution of tabula rasa. Journal of Social and Biological Structures, 6, 101–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden, C. J. ( 1984 a). Parent-offspring conflict over the transmission of culture. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5, 111–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden, C. J. (1984 b). Dual inheritance in haploid organisms: a model of magnetotactic bacteria. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 111, 11–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden, C. J. (1985). Color categorization: a possible concordance between genes and culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 82, 5805–5808.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden, C.J., and Gushurst, A. (1985). Gene-culture coevolution: humankind in the making. In J.H. Fetzer (Ed.), Sociobiology and Epistemology (pp. 3–28 ). Boston, MA: D. Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden, C.J., and Wilson, E.O. (1980). Translation of epigenetic rules of individual behavior into ethnographic patterns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 77, 4382–4386.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden, C.J., and Wilson, E.O. ( 1981 a). Genes, mind, and culture: the coevolutionary process. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden, C.J., and Wilson, E.O. ( 1981 b). Genes, mind, and ideology. The Sciences, 21, 6–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden, C.J., Wilson, E.O. (1982). Mind and the linkage between genes and culture: a précis of Genes, mind, and culture. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden, C.J., and Wilson, E.O. (1983). Promethean fire: reflections on the origin of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mailer, O., and Desor, J.A. (1974). Effect of taste on ingestion by human newborns. In J. Bos-ma (Ed.), Fourth symposium on oral sensation and perception: development in the fetus and infant, pp. 279–311. Washington DC: Government Printing House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M., and Seibert, J. (1976). Classes and collections: internal organization and resulting holistic properties. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 561–577.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marks, I. M. (1969). Fears and phobias. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marks, J., Staski, E., and Schiffer, M.B. (1983). A return to basics. Nature, 302,15–16. McClearn, G. E., and Defries, J. C. ( 1973 ). Introduction to behavioral genetics. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gene-Culture Coevolution: Culture and Biology in Darwinian Perspective 41

    Google Scholar 

  • Mealey, L. (1985). The relationship between social status and biological success: a case study of Mormon religious hierarchy. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6, 249–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medin, D. L., and Smith, E. E. (1984). Concepts and concept formation. Annual Review of Psychology, 35, 113–138.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mollon, J.D. (1982). Color vision. Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 41–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montague, A. (Ed.) (1980). Sociobiology examined. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, G.A., and Riccuiti, H.N. (1973). Infants’ response to strangers during the first year.

    Google Scholar 

  • In L.J. Stone, H. T. Smith and L.B. Murphy (Eds.), The competent infant: research and commentary, pp. 1128–1138, New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, G. P. (1949). Social structure: New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, G. P. (1967). Ethnographic atlas: a summary. Ethnology, 6, 109–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naroll, R., and Cohen, R. (Eds.) (1970). A handbook of method in cultural anthropology. Garden City, NY.: The Natural History Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., and Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ.: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1981). What is language, that a child may learn it, and a child, that he may learn language? Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 23, 90–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., and Keele, S. W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology77, 353–363.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rakic, P. (1979). Genetic and epigenetic determinants of local neuronal circuits in the mammalian central nervous system. In F. O. Schmitt and F. G. Worden (Eds.), The neurosciences: fourth study program (pp 109–127 ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratliff, F. (1976). On the psychophysiological basis of universal color terms. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 120, 311–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1973). Natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 328–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1975). Universals and cultural specifics in human categorization. In R. W. Brishin, S. Bochner and W. J. Lonner (Eds.), Cross-cultural perspectives on learning (pp 177–206 ). New York: Halsted Press, Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E., Mervis, C.B., Gray, W.D., Johnson, D. M., and Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, A. (1980). Sociobiology and the preemption of social science. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sahlins, M. (1976). The use and abuse of biology: an anthropological critique of sociobiology. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salk, L. (1973). The role of the heartbeat in the relations between mother and infant. Scientific American, 228, 24–29.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sandell, J.H., Gross, C. G., and Bornstein, M. H. (1979). Color categories in macaques. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 93, 626–635.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, L., and Bonjean, C. M. (1973). The idea of culture in the social sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. M., and Ewald, R. H. (1968). Culture and society. New York: Ronald.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shannon, C. E. (1948). The mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 27, 379–423, 623–656.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepher, J. (1971). Mate selection among second-generation kibbutz adolescents and adults: incest avoidance and negative imprinting. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 1, 293–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepher, J. (1973). Incest: a biosocial view. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1979). Models of thought. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1981). The sciences of the artificial ( 2nd ed. ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, R. B. (1973). Introduction to cultural anthropology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon. Tobias, P. V. (1981). The evolution of the human brain, intellect and spirit. 1st Abbie Memorial Lecture, University of Adelaide, South Australia, 12th October, 1979. Adelaide: University of Adelaide Information Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trigg, R. (1982). The shaping of man: philosophical aspects of sociobiology. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Berghe, P. L. (1980). Royal incest and inclusive fitness. American Ethnologist, 7, 300–317.

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Berghe, P. L. (1983). Human inbreeding avoidance: culture in nature. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 91–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wexler, K., and Culicover, P.W. (1980). Formal principles of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, L.A. (1949). Ethnological theory. In: R. W. Sellars, V.J. McGill and M. Farber (Eds.), Philosophy for the future (pp 357 — 384 ). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitfield, T.W.A. (1980). Salient features of color space. Perception and Psychophysics, 29, 87–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wickelgren, W. A. (1979). Cognitive psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, T. R. (1972). Introduction to socialization: human culture transmitted. St. Louis, MO: C. V. Mosby.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R. S. (1978). Synchronies in mental development: an epigenetic perspective. Science, 202, 939–948.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witkowski, S. R., and Brown, C. H. (1977). An explanation for color nomenclature universals. American Anthropologist, 79, 50 — 57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, A.P. (1968). Adopt a daughter-in-law, marry a sister: a Chinese solution to the problem of incest taboo. American Anthropologist, 70, 864–874.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, A. P., and Huang, C. S. (1980). Marriage and adoption in China, 1845–1945. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1988 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Lumsden, C.J. (1988). Gene-Culture Coevolution: Culture and Biology in Darwinian Perspective. In: de Kerckhove, D., Lumsden, C.J. (eds) The Alphabet and the Brain. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-01093-8_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-01093-8_2

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-662-01095-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-662-01093-8

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics