Abstract

The printed words you are reading now are the perceptible cornerstones of an otherwise invisible grammatical edifice that is automatically reconstructed in your mind. According to many psycholinguists, comprehending spoken, written or signed sentences involves building grammatical structures. This cognitive activity, usually called syntactic analysis or sentence parsing, includes assigning a word class (part-of-speech) to individual words, combining them into word groups or ‘phrases’, and establishing syntactic relationships between word groups. All these parsing decisions should harmonize not only with rules of grammar but also with the message intended by speaker, writer or signer. Although usually proceeding effortlessly and automatically, the parsing process may slow down, err, or even break down completely when the sentence is very long or contains difficult grammatical constructions. Characterizing the exact nature of such problems and explaining them in terms of underlying cognitive mechanisms are important objectives of the subfield of psycholinguistics called Human Sentence Processing (HSP).

Keywords

Posit Aphasia Ambi Jeanine 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abney, S.P. (1989). A computational model of human parsing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18, 129–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Altmann, G.T.M. & Steedman, M. (1988). Interaction with context during human sentence processing. Cognition, 30, 191–238.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bach, E., Brown, C. & Marslen-Wilson, W. (1986). Crossed and nested dependencies in German and Dutch: A psycholinguistic study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4, 249–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bader, M. & Lasser, I. (1994). German verb-final clauses and sentence processing: Evidence for immediate attachment. In C. Clifton, L. Frazier & K. Rayner (eds.), Perspectives on sentence processing. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  5. Bever, T.G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In J.R. Hayes (ed.), Cognition and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  6. Branigan, H.P., Pickering, M.J., Liversedge, S.P., Stewart, A.J. & Urbach, T.P. (1995). Syntactic priming: Investigating the mental representation of language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24, 489–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brysbaert, M., Mitchell, D.C. & Grondelaers, S. (submitted). Cross-linguistic differences in modifier attachment biases: Evidence against Gricean an tuning accounts.Google Scholar
  8. Caplan, D., Baker, C. & Dehaut, F. (1985). Syntactic determinants of sentence comprehension in aphasia. Cognition, 21, 117–175.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Caplan, D. & Hildebrandt, N. (1988). Disorders of syntactic comprehension. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  10. Christiansen, M.H. & Chater, N. (in press). Connectionist natural language processing: The state of the art. Cognitive Science. Google Scholar
  11. Crain, S. & Steedman, M. (1985). On not being led up the garden path: The use of context by the psychological syntax processor. In D. Dowty, L. Karttunen & A. Zwicky (eds.), Natural language parsing: Psychological, computational and theoretical perspectives. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Crocker, M.W. (1996). Computational psycholinguistics: An interdisciplinary approach to the study of language. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  13. Cuetos, F. & Mitchell, D.C. (1988). Cross-linguistic differences in parsing: Restrictions on the use of the late closure strategy in Spanish. Cognition, 30, 73–105.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dijkstra, T. & De Smedt, K. (1996). Computational psycholinguistics: Symbolic and subsymbolic models of language processing. London: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
  15. Elman, J.L. (1995). Representation and structure in connectionist models. In G.T.M. Altmann (ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational perspectives. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  16. Fodor, J.A., Bever, T.G. & Garrett, M.F. (1974). The psychology of language. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  17. Frazier, L. & Clifton, C. (1996). Construal. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  18. Frazier, L. & Fodor, J.D. (1978). The sausage machine: A new two-stage parsing model. Cognition, 6, 291–325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Frazier, L. & Rayner, K. (1982). Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 178–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Frazier, L., Taft, L., Roeper, T., Clifton C. & Ehrlich, K. (1984). Parallel structure: A source of facilitation in sentence comprehension. Memory and Cognition, 12, 421–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Friedend, A.D. (1997). Diagnosis and reanalysis: Two processing steps the brain may differentiate. In J.D. Fodor & F. Ferreira (eds.), Reanalysis in sentence processing. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  22. Garrod, S.C. & Sanford, A.J. (1994). Resolving sentences in a discourse context: How discourse representation affects language understanding. In M.A. Gernsbacher (ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  23. Gernsbacher, M.A. (1994). Handbook of psycholinguistics. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  24. Gibson, E. (1997). Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. MIT manuscript.Google Scholar
  25. Gorrell, P. (1995). Syntax and parsing. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Haarmann, H.J., Just, M.A. & Carpenter, P.A. (in press). Aphasic sentence comprehension as a resource deficit: A computational approach. Brain and Language. Google Scholar
  27. Jurafsky, D. (1996). A probabilistic model of lexical and syntactic access and disambiguation. Cognitive Science, 20, 137–194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Just, M.A. & Carpenter, P.A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122–149.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kaan, E. (1997). Processing subject-object ambiguities in Dutch. PhD Thesis, University of Groningen. [Groningen Dissertations in Linguistics 20].Google Scholar
  30. Kempen, G. (1996). Computational models of syntactic processing in human language comprehension. In T. Dijkstra & K. De Smedt (eds.), Computational psycholinguistics: Symbolic and subsymbolic models of language processing. London: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
  31. Kimball, J. (1973). Seven principles of surface structure parsing in natural language. Cognition, 2, 15–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Konieczny, L., Hemforth, B., Scheepers, C. & Strube, G. (1997). The role of lexical heads in parsing: Evidence from German. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 307–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Levelt, W.J.M. (1978). A survey of studies in sentence perception: 1970–1976. In W.J.M. Levelt & G.B. Flores d’Arcais (eds.), Studies in the perception of language. Chicester: Wiley.Google Scholar
  34. MacDonald, M.C., Just, M.A. & Carpenter, P.A. (1992). Working memory constraints on the processing of syntactic ambiguity. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 56–98.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. MacDonald, M.C., Pearlmutter, N.J. & Seidenberg, M.S. (1994). Lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, 101, 676–703.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Marcus, M.P (1980). A theory of syntactic recognition for natural language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  37. Marslen-Wilson, W. (1975). Sentence perception as an interactive parallel process. Science, 189, 226–228.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. McClelland, J.L. (1987). The case for interactions in language processing. In M. Coltheart (ed.), Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading. Hove UK: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  39. McRae, K., Spivey-Knowlton, M.J. & Tanenhaus, M.K. (in press). Modeling the influence of thematic fit (and other constraints) in on-line sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language. Google Scholar
  40. Miller, G.A. & Isard, S.D. (1964). Some perceptual consequence of linguistic rules. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 217–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Mitchell, D.C. (1994). Sentence parsing. In M.A. Gernsbacher (ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  42. Pritchett, B.L. (1991). Head position and parsing ambiguity. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20, 251–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Pritchett, B.L. (1992). Grammatical competence and parsing performance. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  44. Stolz, W.S. (1967). A study of the ability to decode grammatically novel sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 867–873.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Tabor, W., Juliano, C. & Tanenhaus, M.K. (1997). Parsing in a dynamical system: An attractor-based account of the interaction of lexical and structural constraints in sentence processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 211–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Tanenhaus, M.K. & Trueswell, J.C. (1995). Sentence comprehension. In J.L. Miller & P.D. Eimas (eds.), Speech language and communication. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  47. Trueswell, J.C. (1996). The role of lexical frequency in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 566–585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Vosse, Th. & Kempen, G. (submitted). Syntactic processing in human sentence comprehension: An inhibition-based parser with a lexicalized grammar.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1998

Authors and Affiliations

  • Gerard Kempen

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations