Skip to main content

Computer Simulation of Social Value Orientation: Vitality, Satisfaction, and Emergent Game Structures

  • Conference paper
Frontiers in Social Dilemmas Research

Abstract

We examined dyadic interaction among eight archetypal social value orientations, or SVOs. (McClintock, 1978). These 8 SVOs differ in terms of two value systems: (1) concern with the well being of others and (2) concern with the well being of self (+,0 for Altruism; +,+ for Cooperation; 0,+ for Individualism; -,+ for Competition; -,0 for Aggression; -,- for Sadomasochism; 0,- for Masochism; and +,- for Martyrdom). All different pairs of SVOs (n=36) were formed, and each dyad played 60,000 2x2 games. For each game, the 2 payoffs in each cell of the matrix were generated randomly so that games had no particular structure (i.e., Prisoner’s Dilemma, Chicken, etc), or symmetry. The decision process was based 0on Kelley and Thibaut’s (1978) notion of transforming the given matrix into the effective matrix. That is, each SVO transformed the pair of given payoffs in each cell to a single number, or effective payoff (utility) based on its SVO (e.g., Cooperators took the sum of the payoffs to self and other; Altruists only considered the payoffs to the partner; Competitors subtracted the other’s points from their own). Next, each SVO chose the row or column with the largest average utility. We examined the long term outcomes of each SVO in two ways: (1) accumulated given points referred to as vitality, and (2) accumulated effective points (utilities) referred to as satisfaction. Vitality was completely determined by two things: (1) player’s concern with its own (given) outcomes, and (2) partner’s concern with player’s (given) outcomes. Although Individualism was the most vital SVO at the single person level, at the dyadic level, pairs of Cooperators were more vital than pairs of Individualists. For Satisfaction, no SVO did best overall. Satisfaction depended completely on the particular SVO pairing. The strongest mutual satisfaction was achieved by SVO pairs with identical effective matrices (e.g., Cooperator/Cooperator, Individualist/Altruist). Emergent Games: We also examined the long-term payoff matrices (the average of given points over 60,000 trials) produced by the pairings of different SVOs. A variety of such Emergent Games occurred, and the structure of each was tied perfectly to the SVO pair that formed it. Thus, the origins of structures are rooted in the social value systems of the involved parties. Notably, the Emergent Game produced by Cooperation versus Individualism is a classic Prisoner’s Dilemma.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic Books. Doi, T. (1990). An experimental investigation of the validity of the characteristic space theory and the measurement of social motivation. The Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29,15–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberger, R., Kuhlman, D. M., &zmp; Cotterell, N. (1992). Effects of social values, effort training, and goal structure on task persistence. Journal of Research in Personality, 26, 258–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griesinger, D. W. & Livingston, J. W. (1973). Toward an interpersonal motivation in experimental games. Behavioral Science, 18,173–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hardin, G. (1968). The tragedy of the commons. Science, 162,1243–1248. Harris, R. J. (1969). A geometric classification system for 2 × 2 interval-symmetric games. Behavioral Science, 14, 138-146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahan, J. P. (1974). Rationality, the prisoner’s dilemma, and population. Journal of Social Issues, 30,189–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, H. H., &zmp; Thibaut, J. W. (1978). Interpersonal relations: A theory of interdependence. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, R. M., McClintock, C. G., & Messick, D. M. (1986). Social values and cooperative response to a simulated resource conservation crisis. Journal of Personality, 54, 576–592.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhlman, D. M., Camac, C. R., &zmp; Cunha, D. A. (1986). Individual differences in social orientation. In H. Wilke, D. Messick, and C. Rutte (Eds.), Experimental social dilemmas (p.151-176). New York: Verlag Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhlman, D. M., &zmp; Marshello, A. F. J. (1975). Individual differences in game motivation as moderators of preprogrammed strategic effects in prisoner’s dilemma. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32,922–931. (a)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhlman, D. M., & Wimberley, D. L. (1976). Expectations of choice behavior held by cooperators, competitors, and individualists across four classes of experimental game. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 69–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liebrand, W. B. G. (1983). A classification of social dilemma games. Simulation and Games, 14, 123–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liebrand, W. B. G. (1984). The effect of social motives, communication and group size on behavior in an N-person multi-stage mixed motive game. European Journal of Social Psychology, 14, 239–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liebrand, W. B. G., & van Run, G. J. (1985). The effects of social motives on behavior in social dilemmas in two cultures. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 86–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liebrand, W. B. G., Wilke, H. A. M., Vogel, R., & Wolters, F. J. M. (1986). Value orientation and conformity. A study using three types of social dilemma games. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 30,77–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maki, J E., & McClintock, C. G. (1983). The accuracy of social value prediction: Actor and observer influences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 829–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClintock, C. G. (1978). Social values: Their definition, measurement and development. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 12, 121–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClintock, C. G., & Allison, S. T. (1989). Social value orientation and helping behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 19, 353–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClintock, C. G., & Liebrand, W. B. G. (1988). Role of interdependence structure, individual value orientation, and another’s strategy in social decision making: A transformational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 396–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Messick, D. M., & Liebrand, W. B. G. (1995). Individual heuristics and the dynamics of cooperation in large groups. Psychological Review, 102, 131–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rapoport, A. (1967). A note on the “index of cooperation” for prisoner’s dilemma. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 11, 101–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rapoport, A., & Chammah, A. M. (1965). Prisoner’s Dilemma. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teta, P. D. (1994). Structural and intrapersonal influences on decision making in 2×2 symmetric games. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Delaware.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tierney, J. (1990). Betting on the Planet. The New York Times Magazine, Dec. 2 (sec. 6), p. 52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Lange, P. A. M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (1994). Social value orientations and impressions of partner’s honesty and intelligence: A test of the might versus morality effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 126–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Vugt, M., Meertens, R. M., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (1995). Car versus public transportation? The role of social value orientations in a real-life social dilemma. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 258–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1996 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this paper

Cite this paper

Joireman, J.A., Shelley, G.P., Teta, P.D., Wilding, J., Michael Kuhlman, D. (1996). Computer Simulation of Social Value Orientation: Vitality, Satisfaction, and Emergent Game Structures. In: Liebrand, W.B.G., Messick, D.M. (eds) Frontiers in Social Dilemmas Research. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-85261-9_16

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-85261-9_16

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-85263-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-85261-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics