Formal Education Versus Everyday Learning

  • Jan J. Elshout
Conference paper
Part of the NATO ASI Series book series (volume 84)

Abstract

Educational philosophy seems to be locked into a pendular motion, in some periods favoring rational formal schooling as its ideal, then swinging to the position that the best of learning is to be found in everyday life. When the latter position is in the ascendance, stress is laid on what formal education does not accomplish and, on the other hand, on the wonderful feats of learning, transfer and performance we may see of persons of who we would not have expected them; not expected, that is, if we take the view that excellence can only result from a transfusion of well designed knowledge from the outside. When, again, formal schooling is in favor, the stress is laid on the beauty of the efficient mass production of knowledgeable people, freed of the ignorance and the irrationality persons left to their own devices are forced to succumb to. One may ask of what energies this swinging motion feeds (e.g. why do we periodically have outbursts of research on the question, whether two learn better than one?). My suggestion is, that it is the combination of the attractiveness with the inconsistency of both positions that makes us feel the tug of the pendulum whenever we get too much committed to one of them. The attractiveness beckons from afar, while to see the inconsistency one has to come close up. Our overall conclusion will be, especially as research on intelligent tutoring systems is concerned, that a pendulum is not a good compass.

Keywords

Decontextualization Transfer 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. 1.
    Adelson, B. (1984). When novices surpass experts: The difficulty of a task may increase with expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 483–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Boschowitsch, L. I. (1970). Die Persönlichkeit und ihre Entwicklung im Schulalter. Berlin: Volk und Wissen Verlag.Google Scholar
  3. 3.
    Bower, G. H., & Hilgard, E. R. (1981). Theories of learning. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Cheng, P. W., & Holyoak, K. J. (1985). Pragmatic reasoning schemas. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 391–416.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Cheng, P. W., Holyoak, K. J., Nisbett, R. E., & Oliver, L. M. (1986). Pragmatic versus syntactic approaches to training deductive reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 293–328.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Cole, M., Gay, J., Glick, J., & Sharp, D. (1971). The cultural context of learning and thinking. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  8. 8.
    Cutting, J. E., & Profitt, D. R. (1982). The minimum principle and the perception of absolute, common, and relative motions. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 211–246.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Fraser, B. J. (1989). Research syntheses on school and instructional effectiveness. International Journal of Educational Research, 13, 707–718.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Griggs, R. A., & Cox, J. R. (1982). The elusive thematic-materials effect in Wason’s selection task. British Journal of Psychology, 73, 407–420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Hayes, J. R., & Simon, H. A. (1977). Psychological differences among problem isomorphs. In N. Castellon, K. Pisoni, & G. Potts (Eds.), Cognitive theory (Vol. 2, pp. 21–41 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  12. 12.
    Holyoak, K. J., & Koh, K. (1987). Surface and structural similarity in analogical transfer. Memory and Cognition, 75, 332–340.Google Scholar
  13. 13.
    Hoogstraten, K. (1976). Alleen of met zijn tweeen. Doctoral dissertation. University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands. [“Alone or Pairwise. Five fieldexperiments with programmed material”. Dutch text with English summary]Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Kulik, J. A., & Kulik, C. C. (1989). Meta-analysis in education. International Journal of Educational Research, 13, 221–340.Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Lehman, D. R., Lempert, R. O., & Nisbett, R. E. (1988). The effects of graduate training on reasoning: Formal discipline and thinking about everyday-life events. American Psychologist, 43, 431–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    McKeachie, W. J. (1974). The decline and fall of the laws of learning. Educational Researcher, 3, 7–11.Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Novick, L. R. (1988). Analogical transfer, problem similarity, and expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogriition, 14, 510–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    Piaget, J. (1972). Intellectual evolution from adolescence to adulthood. Human Development, 25, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    Raaheim, K. (1988). Intelligence and task novelty. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  20. 20.
    Resnick, L. B. (1989). Introduction. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning, and instruction. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  21. 21.
    Skinner, B. F. (1954). The science of learning and the art of teaching. Harvard Educational Review, 24, 86–97.Google Scholar
  22. 22.
    Skinner, B. F. (1958). Teaching machines. Science, 128, 967–977.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. 23.
    Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. In R. W. Riebes & A.S. Carton (Eds.), The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky: Vol. 1. Problems of general psychology. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  24. 24.
    Wason, P. C., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1972). Psychology of reasoning: Structure and content Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  25. 25.
    Webb, N. M. (Ed.) (1989). Peer interaction, problem solving, and cognition: Multidisciplinary perspectives (special issue). International Journal of Educational Research, 13, 1–119.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1992

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jan J. Elshout
    • 1
  1. 1.Laboratory of PsychologyUniversity of AmsterdamAmsterdamThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations