Skip to main content

Relations Between Perception and Action: Unity in Diversity

  • Chapter
Relationships Between Perception and Action
  • 332 Accesses

Abstract

Professor Bieri has no story of his own to tell about perception and action. Nor do I. What is more, I do not even have an axe to grind. Having been raised in the information-processing approach myself, I saw nothing objectionable in its adoption by some of the contributors to this book. But now that Bieri has alerted us to its dangers and insufficiencies, I believe that a few words are in order, not in defense of the overall approach, but on some specific points raised by my co-commentator.

Prepartion of this chapter was helped by a fellowship from the Stiftung Volkswagenwerk.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Ach, N. (1905). Ãœber die Willenstcitigkeit und das Denken. Göttingen: Vandenhoeck Ruprecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ach, N. (1935). Analyse des Willens. Berlin: Urban Schwarzenberg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D.E. (1958). Perception and communication. London: Pergamon.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Brozek, J. (1979). F.C. Donders (1818–1889) and the timing of mental operations. Psychological Reports, 26, 563–569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danto, A.C. (1965). Basic actions. American Philosophical Quarterly, 2, 141–148

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1976). Hierarchical organization: A candidate principle for ethology. In: P.P.G. Bateson R.A. Hinde (Eds.). Growing points in ethology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D.C. (1981). Brainstorms: Philosophical essays on mind and psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dretske, F. (1988). The stance stance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 511–512

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, C.R. (1980). The organization of action: A new synthesis. Hillsdale, NJ.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, A.R. Gardner, B.T. (1988). Feedforward versus feedbackward: An ethólogical alternative to the law of effect. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 429–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J.J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton–Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatfield, G. (1988). Representation and content in some (actual) theories of perception. Studies in the History and Philosophy of Science, 19, 175–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K.S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In: L.A. Jeffress (Ed.) Cerebral mechanisms in behavior, (pp. 112–146 ). New York: Wiley

    Google Scholar 

  • Lotze, R.H. (1852). Medicinische Psychologie oder Physiologie der Seele. Leipzig: Weidmannsche Buchhandlung.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D.G. (1973). Aspects of the theory of comprehension, memory and attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 22–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D.G. (1987). The organization of perception and action: A theory for language and other cognitive skills. Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G.A., Galanter, E., Pribram, K.H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pattee, H.H. (1977). Dynamic and linguistic modes of complex systems. International Journal of General Systems, 3, 259–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1987). Ideo–motor action. In: H. Heuer A.F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on Perception and Action, (pp. 47–76 ) Hillsdale, NJ.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z.M. (1984). Computation and cognition: Toward a foundation for cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, E.S. (1982). An outline of a theory of action systems. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14, 98–134.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roitblat, H.L. (1982). The meaning of representation in animal memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 353–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheerer, E. (1984). Motor theories of cognitive structure: A historical review. In: W. Prinz A.F. Sanders (Eds.) Cognition and motor processes, (pp. 77–98 ). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Scheerer, E. (1988). Muscle sense and innervation feelings: A chapter in the history of perception and action. In: H. Heuer A.F. Sanders (Eds.) Perspectives on perception and action (pp. 171–194 ). Hillsdale, NJ.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheerer, E. (1988). Towards a history of cognitive science, international Social Science Journal, 40 (1), 7–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Hoisten, C., Rönnqvist, L. (1988). Preparation for grasping an object: a developmental study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 610–621.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Holst, E. (1939). Die relative Koordination als Phänomen und als Methode zentralnervöser Funktionsanalyse. Ergebnisse der Physiologie, 42, 228–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Holst, E., Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). Das Reafferenzprinzip ( Wechselwirkungen zwischen Zentralnervensystem und Peripherie ). Naturwissenschaften, 37, 464–476.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Uexküll, J. (1973). Theoretische Biologie, Frankfurt: Suhrkamp (original woik published 1920 ).

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Weizsäcker, V. (1973). Der Gestaltkreis. Frankfurt/M.: Suhrkamp (original work published 1940 ).

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, P. (1941). Self-differentiation of the basic patterns of coordination. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 77 (4). 387

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1911). Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie (6th ed.), Vol. 3. Leipzig: Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1990 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Scheerer, E. (1990). Relations Between Perception and Action: Unity in Diversity. In: Neumann, O., Prinz, W. (eds) Relationships Between Perception and Action. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-75348-0_14

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-75348-0_14

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-75350-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-75348-0

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics