Skip to main content

Outline of Left-Associative Grammar

  • Chapter
Computation of Language

Part of the book series: Symbolic Computation ((1064))

  • 100 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter outlines a grammar which satisfies the Criteria of Psychological Well-Foundedness. Section 3.1 presents a syntactic theory which satisfies the Derivational Order Hypothesis. Section 3.2 illustrates how the syntactic analysis of Left-Associative Grammar is displayed by the associated NEWCAT parsers. Section 3.3 describes a semantic interpretation for this syntax which satisfies the Criteria of Psychological Adequacy. Section 3.4 illustrates the system with the analysis of a “garden path sentence.” Section 3.5 summarizes the descriptive, methodological and heuristic advantages of left-associative grammar.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. The time-linear derivation order characteristic of LA-grammar must be distinguished from Hock-ett’s (1966) notion of a “linear generative grammar,” which is simply a phrase structure grammar without transformations.

    Google Scholar 

  2. The Newcat derivation of 3.1.3 is given in 3.2.2.

    Google Scholar 

  3. See Section 6.2 for the algebraic definition of LA-grammar.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Obviously, LA-grammar is not restricted to Lisp; the rules of an LA-grammar can be parsed in any general purpose programming language. For example, there exists a version of LA-grammar in Prolog.

    Google Scholar 

  5. The rule r-2 is part of the LA-grammar for the context sensitive language akbkck defined in 6.4.3.

    Google Scholar 

  6. The rule-compiler was implemented by Gerald Klix (University of Munich). For use on a PC, the LA-grammar shell was translated into PC-SCHEME because it is considerably smaller than Golden Common Lisp.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Structured lists are easier to print and much faster to display than tree structures.

    Google Scholar 

  8. This category represents a V (sentential expression containing the finite verb) which still needs a D (dative) and an A (accusative) to become a complete sentence.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Examples of ungrammatical sentences are marked with an asterisk (“*”), a convention which dates back at least to Bloomfield (1933).

    Google Scholar 

  10. Since Montague Grammar doesn’t construct a bidirectional surface-meaning mapping, it also fails to satisfy the Criteria of Procedural Adequacy (2.4.1).

    Google Scholar 

  11. See Montague (1974), pp. 222 – 246.

    Google Scholar 

  12. E.g., a semantic type or a kind of model-theoretic denotation.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Hausser (1984a).

    Google Scholar 

  14. For example, we don’t have to build a minimal database from the tree, because the structure was built as a database in the first place.

    Google Scholar 

  15. That the hierarchial analyses of LA-grammar use slot names like SUBJ, DIR-OBJ, etc., is partly a concession to popular usage, and partly intended to distinguish the semantic representation from the syntactic analysis, which uses cases.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Carbonell and Joseph (1986), Nyberg (1988).

    Google Scholar 

  17. If a slot does not yet exist, add-to-value will create it.

    Google Scholar 

  18. The first digit also shows which word contributed what to the semantic representation. For example, the filler “SG-4” (for singular) in 3.3.5 has the word-number 4, because this aspect of the noun phrase was determined by word 4.

    Google Scholar 

  19. This distinction of frames representing different readings is based on a frame copying function which was written by Jaime Carbonell as an addition to the FrameKit software.

    Google Scholar 

  20. This sentence has the same meaning as The man read that a book was given to Mary.

    Google Scholar 

  21. The first digit of SENT-3_3_3 is “3” because this frame is derived from the third word in the sentence, i.e. read.

    Google Scholar 

  22. The third index digit in 3.4.5 is 4 rather than 3 because 3.4.5 is a new derivation, for the sake of demonstration, thus incrementing the sentence counter by 1.

    Google Scholar 

  23. In this way the number of readings is apparent when the first parse appears on the screen.

    Google Scholar 

  24. At present, LA-grammar treats all possible continuations as equal. However, as explained in Hausser (1986), p. 50, a weighing of readings based on the order of rules in the rule packages would be straightforward.

    Google Scholar 

  25. See Sections 5.4 and 5.5 for further discussion.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Berwick & Weinberg (1984), p. 41.

    Google Scholar 

  27. The computational complexity of LA-grammer is analyzed in Chapter 10. 33 See the analysis and discussion of a “garden path” sentence in Section 3.4.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Hausser, R. (1989). Outline of Left-Associative Grammar. In: Computation of Language. Symbolic Computation. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-74564-5_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-74564-5_4

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-74566-9

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-642-74564-5

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics