Skip to main content

Measuring the Behaviour of Dogs: An Ethological Approach

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Domestic Dog Cognition and Behavior

Abstract

What are ‘dog cognition’ studies actually studying? What role does the dog play in behaviour research? In this essay we consider how to study this species from the ethologist’s perspective by providing a critical summary of the various approaches and explaining how these can answer questions on function, evolution, mechanism, and development and by highlighting the potential pitfalls in methodology. It is impossible to claim that one dog is more dog-like than others and it is now evident that even the concept of breed per se presents some problematic issues from the canid ethologist’s perspective. Thus for any sampling it is fundamental to keep in mind what is the research question and to choose the subjects according to what aspects are expected to be relevant. In general the researcher should include a wide range of purebred dogs and mongrels living in human families in a representative sample. When sampling for investigations comparing wolves and dogs, we must bear in mind that dogs present a mosaic pattern of wolf-like traits and cannot be ranked along a strict continuum when assessing their differences from wolves. Therefore for comparative studies and also when the research question regards general dog abilities, a mixed sample of purebred dogs and mongrels is advisable. It is probably even more important to ensure that all animals have had similar past experience, especially with regard to humans. It should be noted that those dog owners that participate on a voluntary basis in a research program are already a specific sub-sample because it is very likely that those owners are particularly interested and take special care of their dogs. Thus it is also likely that these owners want to be part of the experiment and these dogs may actually also ‘need’ the presence of the owner. If the experiment is designed carefully the presence of the owner should not interfere with the outcome. The lack of a generally accepted ethogram is hindering behaviour research on dogs. The scientific community should aim for developing a categorical list of behaviour units that forms the basis of behavioural observations and experimental work.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 189.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1969). Object relations, dependency and attachment: A theoretical review of the infant-mother relationship. Child Development, 40, 969–1025.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barthélémy, I., Barrey, E., Thibaud, J., Uriarte, A., Voit, T., et al. (2009). Gait analysis using accelerometry in dystrophin-deficient dogs. Neuromuscular Disorders, 19, 788–796.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, P. & Martin, P. (1986). Measuring behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw, J. W. S., Blackwell, E. J., & Casey, R. A. (2009). Dominance in domestic dogs—useful construct or bad habit? Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 4, 135–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coppinger, R. P. & Coppinger, L. (2001). Dogs: A startling new understanding of canine origin, behavior and evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fatjó J., Feddersen-Petersen, D., Ruiz de la Torre, J-R., Amat, M., Mets, M., Braus, B, & Manteca, X. (2007). Ambivalent signals during agonistic interactions in a captive wolf pack. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 105, 274–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feuerbacher, E., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2012). Relative efficacy of human social interaction and food as reinforcers for domestic dogs and hand-reared wolves. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 98, 105–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fondon, J. W. & Garner, H. R. (2004). Molecular origins of rapid and continuous morphological evolution. In Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 28, 18058–18063.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, M. G. & Frank, H. (1988). Food reinforcement versus social reinforcement in timber wolf pups. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 26, 467–468.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fugazza, C., & Miklósi, A. (2013). Deferred imitation and declarative memory in domestic dogs. Animal Cognition (on line) doi:10.1007/s10071-013-0656-5

  • Fugazza, C., & Miklósi, A. (submitted). Should old dog trainers learn new tricks? The effectiveness of the Do as I Do and Shaping/Clicker training method in dog training

    Google Scholar 

  • Gácsi, M., Maros, K., Sernkvist, S., Faragó, T., & Miklósi, Á. (2013). Human analogue safe haven effect of the owner: Behavioural and heart rate response to stressful social stimuli in dogs. PLoS ONE, 8(3), e58475

    Google Scholar 

  • Gácsi, M., Győri, B., Virányi, Z., Kubinyi, E., Range F. Belényi, B., et al. (2009a). Explaining dog wolf differences in utilizing human pointing gestures: Selection for synergistic shifts in the development of some social skills. PLoS ONE, 4(8), e6584

    Google Scholar 

  • Gácsi, M., McGreevy P., Kara, E., & Miklósi, A. (2009b). Effects of selection for cooperation and attention in dogs. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 5, 31

    Google Scholar 

  • Gácsi, M., Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., Dóka, A., & Csányi, V. (2001). Attachment behaviour of adult dogs (Canis familiaris) living at rescue centres: Forming new bonds. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115, 423–431

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazit, I., & Terkel, J., (2003). Explosives detection by sniffer dogs following strenuous physical activity. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 81, 149–161

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerencsér, L., Vásárhelyi, G., Nagy, N. Vicsek, T., & Miklósi, Á. (2013). Identification of behaviour in freely moving dogs (Canis familiaris) using inertial sensors. PLoS

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C., & Tomasello, M. (2002). The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science, 298, 1634–1636.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B., Rosati, A., Kaminski, J., Brauer, J., & Call, J. (2009). The domestication hypothesis for dogs’ skills with human communication: A response to Udell et al. (2008) and Wynne et al. (2008). Animal Behaviour, 47, e1–e6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hegedüs, D., Bálint, A., Miklósi, Á., & Pongrácz, P. (2013). Owners fail to influence the choices of dogs in a two-choice, visual pointing task. Behaviour, 150, 427–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirsch-Pasek, K., & Treiman, R. (1981). Doggerel: Motherese in a new context. Journal of Child Language, 9, 229–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, L., Range, F., & Huber, L. (2013). Dogs’ attention towards humans depends on their relationship, not only on social familiarity. Animal Cognition, 16, 435–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hooff, van J. A. R. A. M., & Wensing, J. (1987). Dominance and its behavioral measures in a captive wolf pack. In H. Frank (ed.), Man and wolf: Advances, issues and problems in captive wolf research, pp. 219–252. Junk, Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, A., Hecht, J., & Dedrick, A. (2013). Smelling more or less: Investigating the olfactory experience of the domestic dog. Learning and Motivation, 44, 207–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huber, L., Range, F., Voelkl, B., Szucsich, A., Virányi, Z., Miklósi, A. (2009). The evolution of imitation: what do the capacities of non- human animals tell us about the mechanisms of imitation? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 2299–2309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irion, D. N., Schaffer, A. L., Famula, T. R., Eggleston, M. L., Hughes, S. S., & Pedersen, N. C. (2003). Analysis of genetic variation in 28 dog breed populations with 100 microsatellite markers. Journal of Heredity, 94, 81–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnen, D., Heuwieser, W., & Fischer-Tenhagen, C. (2013). Canine Scent Detection–Fact or Fiction? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 148, 201–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Fischer, J. (2004). Word learning in the domestic dog: Evidence for ‘fastmapping’. Science, 304, 1682–1683.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski, J. Tempelmann, S. Call, J., & Tomasello, M (2009). Domestic dogs comprehend human communication with iconic signs. Developmental Science, 12, 831–837.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kampis, G. Y., Miklósi, Á., Virányi Z., & Gulyás, L. (2010). Video deep tagging and data archiving in the comparative mind database. Proceedings of Measuring Behavior 2010 (Eindhoven, The Netherlands, August 2427), pp. 185–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. (1982). Single-case research designs. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinghammer, E. & Goodman, P.A. (1987). Socialization and management of wolves in captivity. In Frank, H., (Ed.), Man and wolf (pp. 31–61). Dordrecht: Junk Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakatos, G., Soproni, K., Dóka, A., & Miklósi, Á. (2009). A comparative approach to dogs’ (Canis familiaris) and human infants’ comprehension of various forms of pointing gestures. Animal Cognition, 12 621–631.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, G., Karlsson, E. K., Perri, A., Webster, M. T., Ho, S. Y. W., et al. (2012). Rethinking dog domestication by integrating genetics, archeology, and biogeography. PNAS, 109, 8878–8883.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levitis, D. A., Lidicker Jr., W. Z., & Freund, G. (2009). Behvioural biologists do not agree on what constitutes behaviour. Animal Behaviour, 78, 103–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lit, L., Schweitzer, J. B. & Oberbauer, A. M. (2011). Handler beliefs affect scent detection dog outcomes. Animal Cognition, 14, 387–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, K. (2012). A comparison of the sensory development of wolves (Canis lupus lupus) and dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) Ethology, 118, 1–11

    Google Scholar 

  • Mariti, C., Ricci, E., Carlone, B., Moore, J.L., Sighieri, C., & Gazzano, A. (2012). Dog attachment to man: A comparison between pet and working dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 8, 135–145

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall-Pescini, S., Valsecchi, P., Petak, I., Accorsi, P. A., & Prato Previde, E. (2008). Does training make you smarter? The effects of training on dogs’ performance in a problem solving task. Behavioural Processes, 78, 449–454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall-Pescini, S., Passalacqua, C., Barnard, S., Valsecchi, P., & Prato-Previde, E. (2009). Agility and search and rescue training differently affects pet dogs’ behaviour in socio-cognitive tasks. Behavioural Processes, 81, 416–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGreevy, P.D., Starling, M., Branson, N. J., Cobb, M. L., & Calnon, D. (2012). An overview of the dog-human dyad and ethograms within it. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 7, 103–117. doi:10.1016/j.jveb.2011.06.001

    Google Scholar 

  • Miklósi, Á., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Virányi, Z., & Csányi, V. (2003). A simple reason for a big difference: Wolves do not look back at humans but dogs do. Current Biology, 13, 763–766.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2004). Comparative social cognition: What can dogs teach us? Animal Behaviour, 67, 995–1004.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miklósi, A. (2007). Dog behaviour evolution and cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miklósi, A. (2014). Dog behaviour evolution and cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Miklósi, A., & Topál, J. (2011). On the hunt for the gene of perspective taking: Pitfalls in methodology. Learning and Behaviour, 39, 310–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, R. W. (2001). Americans’ talk to dogs: Similarities and differences with talk to infants. Research on Language and Social Interactions, 34, 183–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mongillo, P., Bono, G., Regolin, L., & Marinelli, L. (2010). Selective attention to humans in companion dogs, Canis familiaris. Animal Behaviour, 80, 1057–1063.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overall, K. L. & Love, M. (2001). Dog bites to humans—dash demography, epidemiology, injury, and risk. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 218, 1923–1934.

    Google Scholar 

  • Packard, J. M. (2003). Wolf behaviour: Reproductive, social and intelligent. In Mech, D. and Boitani, L., (Eds.), Wolves: Behavior, Ecology and Conservation (pp. 35–65). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pal, S. K., Ghosh, B., & Roy, S. (1998). Dispersal behaviour of free-ranging dogs (Canis familiaris) in relation to age, sex, season, and dispersal distance. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 62, 123–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pal, S. K. (2010). Play behaviour during early ontogeny in free-ranging dogs (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 126, 140–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pal, S. K. (2008). Maturation and development of social behaviour during early ontogeny in free-ranging dog puppies in West Bengal, India. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 111, 95–107

    Google Scholar 

  • Parker, H. G. (2012). Genomic analyses of modern dog breeds. Mammalian Genome, 23, 19–27

    Google Scholar 

  • Passlacqua, C., Marshall-Pescini, S., Barnard, S., Lakatos, G., Valsecchi, P., & Prato Previde, E. (2011). Human-directed gazing behaviour in puppies and adult dogs, Canis lupus familiaris. Animal Behaviour, 82, 1043–1050

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfungst, O. (1911). Clever hans (The horse of Mr. von Osten): A contribution to experimental animal and human psychology (Trans. C.L. Rahn). New York, NY: Henry Holt. (Originally published in German, 1907)

    Google Scholar 

  • Pilley, J. W., (2013). Border collie comprehends sentences containing a prepositional object, verb, and direct object. Learning and Motiviation, 44, 229–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pongrácz, P., Miklósi, Á., Vida, V., & Csányi, V. (2005). The pet-dogs’ ability for learning from a human demonstrator in a detour task is independent from the breed and age. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 90, 309–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pongrácz, P., Gácsi, M., Hegedüs, D., Péter, A., & Miklósi, Á. (2013a). Test sensitivity is important for detecting variability in pointing comprehension in canines. Animal Cognition, 16, 721–735.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pongrácz, P., Hegedűs, D., Sanjurjo, B., Kővári, A., & Miklósi, Á. (2013b). “We will work for you”—Social influence may suppress individual food preferences in a communicative situation in dogs. Learning and Motivation, 44, 270–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prato-Previde, E., Fallani, G., & Valsecchi, P. (2006). Gender differences in owners interacting with pet dogs: An observational study. Ethology, 112, 64–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prato-Previde, E., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Valsecchi, P. (2008). Is your choice my choice? The owners’ effect on pet dogs’ (Canis lupus familiaris) performance in a food choice task. Animal Cognition, 11, 167–174

    Google Scholar 

  • Preston, T., Baltzer, W., & Trost, S. (2012). Accelerometer validity and placement for detection of changes in physical activity in dogs under controlled conditions on a treadmill. Research in Veterinary Science, 93, 412–416

    Google Scholar 

  • Schenkel, R. (1947). Ausdrucks-Studien an Wölfen. Behaviour, 81–129

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, J. P. & Fuller, J. L. (1965). Genetics and the social behaviour of the dog. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Somppi, S., Törnqvist, H., Hänninen, L., Krause, C., & Vainio, O. (2011). Dogs do look at images: Eye tracking in canine cognition research. Journal of Veterinary Behaviour: Clincal applications and Research, 6, 64–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Svartberg (2005). A comparison of behaviour and everyday life: Evidence of three consistent boldness-related personality traits in dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 91, 103–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szetei, V., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J, & Csányi, V. (2003). When dogs seem to lose their nose: An investigation on the use of visual and olfactory cues in communicative context between dog and owner. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 83, 141–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tami, G. & Gallagher (2009). Description of the behaviour of domestic dog (Canis familiaris) by experienced and inexperienced people. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 120, 159–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Téglás, E., Gergely, A., Kupán, K., Miklósi, Á., & Topál, J. (2012). Dogs’ gaze following is tuned to human communicative signals. Current Biology, 22, 209–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 20, 410–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., & Csányi, V. (1997). Dog-human relationship affects problem solving behaviour in the dog. Anthrozoös, 10, 214–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., & Csányi, V. (1998). Attachment behaviour in dogs: A new application of Ainsworth’s (1969) Strange Situation Test. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112, 219–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Topál, J., Byrne, R.W., Miklosi, A., & Csányi, V. (2006). Reproducing human actions and action sequences: “Do as I do!” in a dog. Animal Cognition, 9, 355–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., Gácsi, M., Dóka, A., Pongrácz, P., Kubinyi, E., et al. (2009). The dog as a model for understanding human social behavior. Advances in the Study of Animal Behaviour, 39, 71–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Udell, M. A. R., Dorey, N. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2008). Wolves outperform dogs in following human social cues. Animal Behaviour, 76, 1767–1773.

    Google Scholar 

  • Udell, M. A. R., Dorey, N. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2010). The performance of stray dogs (Canis familiaris) living in a shelter on human-guided object-choice tasks. Animal Behaviour, 79, 717–725.

    Google Scholar 

  • Udell, M. A. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2011). Reevaluating canine perspective-taking behavior. Learning and Behaviour, 39, 318–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Utrata, E., Viranyi, Z., & Range, F. (2012). Quantity discrimination in wolves (Canis lupus) Frontiers in Psychology. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00505

  • Virányi, Z., Gácsi, M., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Belényi, B., Ujfalussy, D., et al. (2008). Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris). Animal Cognition, 11, 373–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wan, M., Kubinyi, E., Miklósi, Á., & Champagne, F. (2009). A cross-cultural comparison of reports by German Shepherd owners in Hungary and the United States of America. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 121, 206–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wemelsfelder, F. (2007). How animals communicate quality of life: The qualitative assessment of animal behaviour. Animal Welfare, 16, 25–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynne, C. D. L., Udell, M. A. R., & Lord, K. A. (2008). Ontogeny’s impact on human-dog communication. Animal Behaviour, 76, e1–e4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrigglesworth, D., Mort, E., Upton, S., & Miller, A. (2011). Accuracy of the use of triaxial accelerometry for measuring daily activity as a predictor of daily maintenance energy requirement in healthy adult Labrador retrievers. American Journal of Veterinary Research, 72, 1151–1155.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

A.M. is grateful for the support of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences (MTA 01 031), and the European Science Foundation (ESF Research Networking Programme titled “The Evolution of Social Cognition: Comparisons and integration across a wide range of human and non-human animal species”). C.F. is grateful for the support of the APDT Foundation and for the fellowship of the Hungarian Scholarship Board.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Claudia Fugazza .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Fugazza, C., Miklósi, Á. (2014). Measuring the Behaviour of Dogs: An Ethological Approach. In: Horowitz, A. (eds) Domestic Dog Cognition and Behavior. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-53994-7_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics