Advertisement

Perception pp 713-729 | Cite as

Time Perception

  • Ernst Pöppel
Part of the Handbook of Sensory Physiology book series (SENSORY, volume 8)

Abstract

The experience of time and how it might be explained has always been a persistent challenge. One of the most compelling analyses of subjective time is the one given by Augustinus (397/398 A.D.) in the 11th book of his “Confessions.” His ideas about the subjective measurement of time are still impressive today; he relates time to the perception of events, an idea that recently has been discussed by Gibson (1973, “Events are perceivable but time is not”). This chapter has been written in order to argue for such a substitution of time perception by event perception. It shall further be argued that in order to deal with human time perception one has to establish a taxonomy of temporal experiences. It appears that different unrelated temporal experiences are often subsumed under the term time perception and that many controversies found in the literature on subjective time stem from an absence of such a taxonomy of temporal experiences.

Keywords

Temporal Integration Time Perception Temporal Organization Storage Size Subjective Time 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Adamson, R.: Anchor effect limits. Psychon. Sci. 9, 179–180 (1967).Google Scholar
  2. Albert, M.L., Bear, D.: Time to understand —a case study of word deafness with reference to the role of time in auditory comprehension. Brain 97, 373–384 (1974).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Allan, L.G., Kristofferson, A.B.: Psychophysical theories of duration discrimination. Perception & Psychophysics 16, 26–34 (1974).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Allen, G.D.: Speech rhythm: its relation to performance universals and articulatory timing. J. Phonetics 3, 75–86 (1975).Google Scholar
  5. Allport, D.A.: Phenomenal simultaneity and the perceptual moment hypothesis. Br. J. Psychol. 59, 395–406 (1968).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Augenstine, L.G.: A model of how humans process information. Biometrics 18, 420–421 (1962).Google Scholar
  7. Augustinus: Confessiones. Book 11. 397/398Google Scholar
  8. Axel, R.: Estimation of time. In: Archives of psychology. i12, 5–77 (1925).Google Scholar
  9. Baer, K.E. von: Welche Auffassung der lebenden Natur ist die richtige? Und wie ist diese Auffassung auf die Entomologie anzuwenden? Speech in St. Petersburg 1860. Schmitzdorff, H. (ed.).St. Petersburg Verlag der kaiserl. Hofbuchhandl. 1864, pp. 237–284.Google Scholar
  10. Bechinger, D., Kongehl, G., Kornhuber, H.H.: Natural 2-second cycle in time perception and human information transmission. Die Naturwissenschaften 56, 419 (1969).Google Scholar
  11. Bindra, D., Waksberg, H.: Methods and terminology in studies of time estimation. Psychol. Bull. 53, 155–159 (1956).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Boring. E.G.: Sensation and perception in the history of experimental psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts 1942.Google Scholar
  13. Carlson. V.R., Feinberg. I.: Time judgment as a function of method, practice, and sex. J. exp. Psychol. 85. 171–180 (1970).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Carmon, A., Nachshon, I.: Effect of unilateral brain damage on perception of temporal order. Cortex 7, 410–418 (1971).Google Scholar
  15. Clausen, J.: An evaluation of experimental methods of time estimation. J. exp. Psychol. 40. 756–761 (1950).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cohen, J.: Psychological time in health and disease. Springfield. Ill.: Charles C.Thomas 1967.Google Scholar
  17. Corkin. S.: Serial-ordering deficits in inferior readers. Neuropsychologia 12, 347–354 (1974).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Creelman, CD.: Human discrimination of auditory duration. J. of the Acoustical Soc. of America 34, 582–593 (1962).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Danziger, K., du Preez, P.D.: Reliability of time estimation by the method of reproduction. Perc. & mot. skills 16, 879–884 (1963).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Donders. F.C.: On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychologica 30, 412–431 (1969). (Orig. 1868).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Doob. L.W.: Patterning of time. New Haven and London: Yale University Press 1971.Google Scholar
  22. Edgell. B., Waller, A.W.: On time judgments. Am. J. Psychol. 14, 154–174 (1903).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Efron, R.: The duration of the present. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 138, 713–729 (1967).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Eisler, H.: Experiments on subjective duration 1868–1975: a collection of power function exponents. Psychol. Bulletin 83. 1154–1171 (1976).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Eriksen, C.W., Collins, J.F.: Sensory traces versus the psychological moment in the temporal organization of form. J. exp. Psychol. 77. 376–382 (1968).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Exner. S.: Experimentelle Untersuchung der einfachsten psychischen Prozesse. Pflügers Archiv ges. Physiol. 11, 403–432 (1875).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Fodor, J., Bever, T.G., Garrett, M.F.: The psychology of language. An introduction to psycholin-guistics and generative grammar. New York: McGraw-Hill 1974.Google Scholar
  28. Fraisse, P.: Perception et estimation du temps. In: Fraisse, P., Piaget, J. Traité de psychologie expérimentale. (eds.). Paris: Presses Universitaires 1963, Vol. VI, pp. 60–95.Google Scholar
  29. Fraisse, P.: The psychology of time. London: Eyre & Spottiswoode 1964.Google Scholar
  30. Fraisse, P., Bonnet, C., Gelly, N., Michon, G.: Vergleich der Zeitschätzungsmethoden. Zeitschrift Psychol. 167. 268–277 (1962).Google Scholar
  31. Frankenhaeuser, M.: Estimation of time. An experimental study. Stockholm: Almqvist and Wiksel 1959.Google Scholar
  32. Getty, D.J.: Discrimination of short temporal intervals: a comparison of two models. Perception and Psychophysics 18. 1–8 (1975).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Gibson. J.J.: Events are perceivable but time is not. Paper presented at a meeting of the International Society for the Study of Time, Japan 1973.Google Scholar
  34. Goldman-Eisler. F.: Sequential temporal patterns and cognitive processes in speech. Acta neurolog-ica and psychiatrica. Belgica 67, 841–851 (1967).Google Scholar
  35. Goldstone, S.: The human clock: a framework for the study of healthy and deviant time perception. Annals New York Acad. Sci. 138, 767–783 (1967).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Gooddy. W.: Disorders of the time sense. In: Handbook of clinical neurology. P.J. Vinken and G.W. Bruyn (eds.). Amsterdam: North Holland Publishing 1969, Vol. III, pp. 229–250.Google Scholar
  37. Gregg, L.W.: Fractionation of temporal intervals. J. exp. Psychol. 42, 307–312 (1951).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Guyau, M.: La genèse de l’idée de temps. Paris: F. Alcan 1890.Google Scholar
  39. Harter, M.R.: Excitability cycles and cortical scanning: a review of two hypotheses of central intermittency in perception. Psychol. Bull. 68, 47–58 (1967).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Harter, R., White, CT.: Periodicity within reaction time distributions and electromyograms. Q. J. exp. Psychol. 20, 157–166 (1968).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Hearnshaw, L.S.: Temporal integration and behaviour. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society 30, 1–20 (1956).Google Scholar
  42. Helson, H.: Adaptation-level theory. New York: Harper and Row 1964.Google Scholar
  43. Hirsh, I.J., Sherrick, C.E., Jr.: Perceived order in different sense modalities. J. exp. Psychol. 62, 423–432 (1961).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Hoagland, H.: The physiological control of judgments of duration: evidence for a chemical clock. J. genet. Psychol. 9, 267–287 (1933).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Hoche, A.: Langeweile. Psychol. Forschung 3, 258–271 (1923).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Holubář, J.: The sense of time. Cambridge, Mass.-London: M.I.T. Press 1969.Google Scholar
  47. Holubář, J., Machek, J.: Time sense and epileptic EEG activity. Epilepsia 3, 323–328 (1962).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Hornstein, A.D., Rotter, G.S.: Research methodology in temporal perception. J. exp. Psychol. 79, 561–564 (1969).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Horst, L. van der: Über die Psychologie des Korsakowsyndroms. Monatsschrift für Psychiatrie und Neurologie 83, 65–84 (1932).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. James, W.: The principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt 1890, Vol. I.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Kent, R.D., Moll, K.L.: Articulatory timing in selected consonant sequences. Brain and Language 2, 304–323 (1975).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Köhler, W.: Zur Theorie des Sukzessivvergleichs und der Zeitfehler. Psychol. Forschung 4, 115–175 (1923).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Kristofferson, A.B.: Attention and psychophysical time. Acta psychol. 27, 93–100 (1967).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Lackner, J.R., Teuber, H.-L.: Alterations in auditory fusion thresholds after cerebral injury in man. Neuropsychologia 11, 409–415 (1973).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Lashley, K.S.: The problem of serial order in behavior. In: Cerebral mechanisms in behavior. Jeffress, L.A. (ed.). New York: Wiley 1951, pp. 112–136.Google Scholar
  56. Latour, P.L.: Evidence of internal clocks in the human operator. Acta psychol. 27, 341–348 (1967).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Mach, E.: Untersuchungen über den Zeitsinn des Ohres. Sitzungsberichte der mathem.-naturwiss. Classe d. Kaiserl. Akademie d. Wissenschaften 51, II. Abt. Heft 1-5, 133–150 (1865).Google Scholar
  58. Mach, E.: Die Analyse der Empfindungen und das Verhältnis des Physischen zum Psychischen. Jena: G. Fischer 1885.Google Scholar
  59. Martin, J.G.: Rhythmic (hierarchical) versus serial structure in speech and other behavior. Psychol. Review 79, 487–509 (1972).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Michon, J.A.: Timing in temporal tracking. Soesterberg: Institute of Perception RVO-TNO 1967.Google Scholar
  61. Michon, J.A.: Processing of temporal information and the cognitive theory of time experience. Studium Generale 23, 249–265 (1970).Google Scholar
  62. Milner, B.: Hemispheric specialization: scope and limits. In: The neurosciences: third study program. Schmitt, F.O., Worden, F.G. (eds.). Cambridge-London: M.I.T. Press 1974, pp. 75–89.Google Scholar
  63. Müller, G.E., Schumann, F.: Experimentelle Beiträge zur Untersuchung des Gedächtnisses. Zeitschrift für Psychologie 6, 81–190 (1894).Google Scholar
  64. Nichols, H.: The psychology of time. Amer. J. Psychol. 3, 453–530 (1891).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Orme, J.E.: Time, experience and behaviour. London: Iliffe Books 1969.Google Scholar
  66. Ornstein, R.E.: On the experience of time. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books 1969.Google Scholar
  67. Podvigin, N.F.: On the mechanism of operative memory in the visual system. Sechenov Physiological Journal of the USSR 58, 592–594 (1972).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Pöppel, E.: Oszillatorische Komponenten in Reaktionszeiten. Naturwissenschaften 55, 449–450 (1968).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Pöppel, E.: Excitability cycles in central intermittency. Psychol. Forschung 34, 1–9 (1970).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Pöppel, E.: Oscillations as possible basis for time perception. In: The study of time. Fraser, J.T., Huber, F.C., Müller, G.H. (eds.). Berlin: Springer 1972, pp. 219–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Pöppel, E.: Influence of pause duration on the reproduction of a 2-second interval. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 2. 291–292 (1973).Google Scholar
  72. Pöppel, E., Cramon, D., von, Backmund, H.: Eccentricity-specific dissociation of visual functions in patients with lesions of the central visual pathways. Nature 256, 489–490 (1975).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Postman, L., Miller, G.A.: Anchoring of temporal judgments. Amer. J. Psychol. 58, 43–53 (1945).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Quasebarth, K.: Zeitschätzung und Zeitauffassung optisch und akustisch ausgefüllter Intervalle. Arch. ges. Psychol. 49, 379–432 (1924).Google Scholar
  75. Richards, W.: Time estimates measured by reproduction. Perceptual and motor skills 18, 929–943 (1964).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Ross, S., Katchmar, L.: The construction of a magnitude function for short time-intervals. Amer. J. Psychol. 64, 397–401 (1951).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Rubin, E.: Some elementary time experiences. Acta psychol. 1, 206–211 (1935).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Shallice, T.: The detection of change and the perceptual moment hypothesis. The British J. of Statistical Psychol. 17, 113–135 (1964).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Stern, L.W.: Psychische Präsenzzeit. Zeitschrift für Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane 13, 325–349 (1897).Google Scholar
  80. Sternberg, S.: Memory scanning: new findings and current controversies. Quart. J. exp. Psychol. 27, 1–32 (1975).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Stevens, S.S.: On the psychophysical law. Psychol. Review 64, 153–181 (1957).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Stroud, J.M.: The fine structure of psychological time. In: Information theory in psychology. Quastler, H. (ed.). Glencoe, I11.: Free Press 1955, pp. 174–205.Google Scholar
  83. Sturt, M.: The psychology of time. London: Kegan Paul 1925.Google Scholar
  84. Swift, E.J., McGeoch, J.A.: An experimental study of the perception of filled and empty time. J. exp. Psychol. 8, 240–249 (1925).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Talland, G.A.: Les troubles de l’oricntation temporelle dans le syndrome amnésique. L’encéphale 50, 450–470 (1961).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Urbantschitsch, V.: Über eine Eigentümlichkeit der Schallempfindungen geringster Intensität. Centralbl. f. d. med. Wissensch. 13, 625–628 (1875).Google Scholar
  87. Vierordt, K.: Der Zeitsinn nach Versuchen. Tübingen: Laupp 1868.Google Scholar
  88. Vroon, P.A.: Effects of presented and processed information on duration experience. Acta Psychol. 34, 115–121 (1970).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Vroon, P.A.: Is there a time quantum in duration experience? Am. J. Psychol. 87, 237–245 (1974).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Wallace, M., Rabin, A.I.: Temporal experience. Psychol. Bull. 57, 213–236 (1960).PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Weber, A.O.: Estimation of time. Psychol. Bull. 30, 233–252 (1933).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Wever, R.: Pendulum versus relaxation oscillation. In: Circadian Clocks. Aschoff, J. (ed.). Amsterdam: North-Holland Publ. 1965, pp. 74–83.Google Scholar
  93. White, C.T.: Temporal numerosity and the psychological unit of duration. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied 77, 1–37 (1963).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Wiener, N.: Time and the science of organization. Scientia 93, 199–205 (1958).Google Scholar
  95. Williams, M., Zangwill, O.L.: Disorders of temporal judgment associated with amnesic states. J. ment. Sci. 96, 484–493 (1950).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Woodrow, H.: The temporal indifference interval determined by the method of mean error. J. exp. Psychol. 17, 167–188 (1934).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Woodrow, H.: Time perception. In: Handbook of experimental psychology. Stevens, S.S. (ed.). New York, London: Wiley 1951, pp. 1224–1236.Google Scholar
  98. Wundt, W.: Ueber psychologische Methoden. Philosophische Studien 1, 1–38 (1881).Google Scholar
  99. Wundt, W.: Einführung in die Psychologie. Leipzig: Voigtländer 1911.Google Scholar
  100. Zurif, E.B., Carson, G.: Dyslexia in relation to cerebral dominance and temporal analysis. Neuropsy-chologia 8, 351–361 (1970).CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag. Berlin Heidelberg 1978

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ernst Pöppel
    • 1
  1. 1.Germany

Personalised recommendations