Semantic Accounts of Computation Examined

Part of the Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics book series (SAPERE, volume 12)

Abstract

This chapter examines some accounts according to which external semantic content is inherent to the computational process, and so any computational explanation entails content ascription to the explanandum. The goal of these accounts is clear: by ascribing semantic content to digital computing systems proper, proponents of the semantic view of computation aim to explain cognition computationally.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Allan, R.A.: A history of the personal computer the people and the technology. Allan Publishing, London (2001)Google Scholar
  2. Bickhard, M.H., Terveen, L.: Foundational issues in artificial intelligence and cognitive science: impasse and solution. Elsevier, Amsterdam (1995)Google Scholar
  3. Burge, T.: Individualism and Psychology. The Philosophical Review 95(1), 3–45 (1986), doi:10.2307/2185131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Clark, A.: Mindware: an introduction to the philosophy of cognitive science. Oxford University Press, New York (2001)Google Scholar
  5. Copeland, B.J.: Computation. In: Floridi, L. (ed.) The Blackwell Guide to the Philosophy of Computing and Information, pp. 3–17. Blackwell, Malden (2004)Google Scholar
  6. Dietrich, E.: Semantics and the computational paradigm in cognitive psychology. Synthese 79(1), 119–141 (1989), doi:10.1007/BF00873258MathSciNetCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Egan, F.: Computation and Content. The Philosophical Review 104(2), 181–203 (1995), doi:10.2307/2185977CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Fodor, J.A.: The language of thought. Harvard University Press, Cambridge (1975)Google Scholar
  9. Fodor, J.A.: Methodological solipsism considered as a research strategy in cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3(01), 63–73 (1980), doi:10.1017/S0140525X00001771CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Fodor, J.A., Pylyshyn, Z.W.: Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis. Cognition 28(1-2), 3–71 (1988), doi:10.1016/0010-0277(88)90031-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Haugeland, J.: Mind design: philosophy, psychology, artificial intelligence, 1st edn. MIT Press, Cambridge (1981)Google Scholar
  12. Haugeland, J.: Artificial intelligence: the very idea. MIT Press, Cambridge (1985)Google Scholar
  13. Haugeland, J.: Mind design II: philosophy, psychology, artificial intelligence (rev. and enl. ed.). MIT Press, Cambridge (1997) Google Scholar
  14. Newell, A.: Physical Symbol Systems. Cognitive Science 4(2), 135–183 (1980a), doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0402_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Newell, A.: The Knowledge Level. AI Magazine 2(2), 1–20 (1980b)MathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  16. Newell, A., Simon, H.A.: Human problem solving. Prentice-Hall (1972)Google Scholar
  17. Newell, A., Simon, H.A.: Computer science as empirical inquiry: symbols and search. Communications of the ACM 19(3), 113–126 (1976), doi:10.1145/360018.360022MathSciNetCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Peacocke, C.: Computation as Involving Content: A Response to Egan. Mind and Language 14(2), 195–202 (1999), doi:10.1111/1468-0017.00109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Piccinini, G.: Computation without Representation. Philosophical Studies 137(2), 205–241 (2008), doi:10.1007/s11098-005-5385-4MathSciNetCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Pylyshyn, Z.W.: Computation and cognition: toward a foundation for cognitive science. The MIT Press, Cambridge (1984)Google Scholar
  21. Pylyshyn, Z.W.: Computing in Cognitive Science. In: Posner, M.I. (ed.) Foundations of Cognitive Science, pp. 49–92. MIT Press, Cambridge (1989)Google Scholar
  22. Pylyshyn, Z.W.: What’s in Your Mind? In: LePore, E., Pylyshyn, Z.W. (eds.) What is Cognitive Science?, pp. 1–25. Blackwell, Malden (1999)Google Scholar
  23. Pylyshyn, Z.W.: Things and places: how the mind connects with the world. MIT Press, Cambridge (2007)Google Scholar
  24. Ramsey, W.: Representation reconsidered. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Scheutz, M.: Computationalism – the next generation. In: Scheutz, M. (ed.) Computationalism: New Directions, pp. 1–22. MIT Press, Cambridge (2002)Google Scholar
  26. Shagrir, O.: What is Computer Science About? The Monist 82(1), 131–149 (1999), doi:10.2307/27903625Google Scholar
  27. Shagrir, O.: Content, computation and externalism. Mind 110(438), 369–400 (2001), doi:10.1093/mind/110.438.369Google Scholar
  28. Smith, B.C.: Formal symbol manipulation: Ontological critique (forthcoming), http://www.ageofsignificance.org (retrieved)
  29. Smith, B.C.: On the origin of objects. MIT Press, Cambridge (1996)Google Scholar
  30. Smith, B.C.: The foundations of computing. In: Scheutz, M. (ed.) Computationalism: New Directions, pp. 23–58. MIT Press, Cambridge (2002)Google Scholar
  31. Smith, B.C.: Age of significance: Introduction (2010), http://www.ageofsignificance.org (retrieved)
  32. Wells, A.J.: Turing’s Analysis of Computation and Theories of Cognitive Architecture. Cognitive Science 22(3), 269–294 (1998), doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2203_1CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Humanities & Languages University of New South WalesSydneyAustralia

Personalised recommendations