Advertisement

Living in the Model: The Cognitive Ecology of Time—A Comparative Study

Conference paper
Part of the Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics book series (SAPERE, volume 8)

Abstract

Time is at once familiar and mysterious, its status in the physical universe being uncertain and contested. Time seems to be fundamental to both biology and to the world of human experience. It seems certain that human beings in all cultures experience time, and have ways of linguistically referring to relations between events in time. It has been proposed by some cognitive scientists that there is a natural, transcultural conceptual domain of time. Cultural conceptions of time, however, vary considerably. I present anthropological linguistic data from a study that my colleagues and I conducted in an indigenous Amazonian community. Concepts of time are cultural and historical constructions, constituted by schematic time interval systems, and embodied in language and culture dependent symbolic cognitive artefacts. “Living in time”, I contend, is to live in a model. Time is both artifactual model and cognitive niche, made possible by the wider biocultural niche of language.

Keywords

Amazonia Anthropology Cognitive artifact Language Niche construction Linguistics Time 

References

  1. 1.
    Newton, I.S.: Philosophiæ Naturalis Principia Mathematica. Royal Society, London (1686)Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Einstein, A.: Relativity: The Special and the General Theory. Methuen, London (1920)Google Scholar
  3. 3.
    McTaggart, J.E.: The unreality of time. Mind Q Rev. Psychol. Philos. 17, 456–473 (1908)Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Smart, J.J.C.: The river of time. Mind New Ser. 58(232), 483–494 (1949)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Whorf, B.L.: Science and linguistics. Technol. Rev. 42(6), 229–231, 247–248 (1940)Google Scholar
  6. 6.
    Whorf, B.L.: An American Indian model of the universe. Int. J. Am. Linguist. 16, 67–72 (1950)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Minkovsky, H.: Space and time. In: Smart, JJC. (ed.) Problems of Space and Time, vol. 6760, p. 927. Macmillan Pub Co, London (1964)Google Scholar
  8. 8.
    Bergson, H.: Time and Free Will: An Essay on the Immediate Data of Consciousness (Transl. F.L. Pogson). Macmillan, London (1910)Google Scholar
  9. 9.
    Whitrow, G.J.: Time in History: Views of Time from Prehistory to the Present Day. Oxford University Press, Oxford (1988)Google Scholar
  10. 10.
    Postill, J.: Clock and calendar time: a missing anthropological problem. Time Soc. 11(2/3), 251–270 (2002)Google Scholar
  11. 11.
    Sinha, C., da Silva Sinha, V., Zinken, J., Sampaio, W.: When time is not space: the social and linguistic construction of time intervals and temporal event relations in an Amazonian culture. Lang. Cogn. 31, 137–169 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    da Silva Sinha, V., Sinha, C., Sampaio, W., Zinken, J.: Event-based time intervals in an Amazonian culture. In: Filipović, L., Jaszczolt, K. (eds.) Space and Time in Languages and Cultures 2: Language, Culture, and Cognition, pp. 15–35. Human Cognitive Processing Series 37. John Benjamins, Amsterdam (2012)Google Scholar
  13. 13.
    Kant, I.: Critique of Pure Reason (N. K. Smith, trans.). Palgrave Macmillan, Basingstoke (1929 [1787])Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Evans-Pritchard, E.E.: Nuer time-reckoning. Afr. J. Int. Afr. Inst. 12(2), pp. 189–216 (1939)Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Evans-Pritchard, E.E.: Nuer. Oxford University Press, Oxford (1940)Google Scholar
  16. 16.
    Whitrow, G.J.: Time in History: Views of Time from Prehistory to the Present Day, p. 15. Oxford University Press, Oxford (1988)Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Clark, H.H.: Space, time, semantics and the child. In: Moore, T.E. (ed.) Cognitive Development and the Acquisition of Language, pp. 27–63. Academic Press, New York (1973)Google Scholar
  18. 18.
    Haspelmath, M.: From Space to Time: Temporal Adverbials in the World’s Languages (Lincom Studies in Theoretical Linguistics 3). Lincom Europa, Munich (1997)Google Scholar
  19. 19.
    Fauconnier, G., Turner, M.: Rethinking metaphor. In: Gibbs, R. (ed.) The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought, pp. 53–66. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Sampaio, W., Sinha, C., da Silva Sinha, V.: Mixing and mapping: motion, path and manner in Amondawa. In Guo, J., Lieven, E., Budwig, N., Ervin-Tripp, S., Nakamura, K., Özçalışkan Ş. (eds.) Crosslinguistic Approaches to the Study of Language. Research in the Tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin, pp. 649–668. Psychology Press, London & New York (2009)Google Scholar
  21. 21.
    Talmy, L.: Lexicalization patterns: semantic structure in lexical forms. In: Shopen, T. (ed.) Language Typology and Syntactic Description. Grammatical Categories and the Lexicon, vol. 3, pp. 36–149. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1985)Google Scholar
  22. 22.
    Shore, B.: Culture in Mind: Cognition, Culture and the Problem of Meaning. Oxford University Press, New York (1996)Google Scholar
  23. 23.
    Casasanto, D.: Space for thinking. In: Evans, V., Chilton, P. (eds.) Language, Cognition and Space: The State of the Art and New Directions, pp. 453–478. Equinox, London (2010)Google Scholar
  24. 24.
    Clark, A.: Language, embodiment and the cognitive niche. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 370–374 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. 25.
    Magnani, L.: Abductive Cognition: The Epistemological and Eco-cognitive Dimensions of Hypothetical Reasoning. Cognitive Systems Monographs, vol. 3. Springer, Berlin (2009)Google Scholar
  26. 26.
    Boivin, N.: Material Cultures, Material Minds: The Role of Things in Human Thought, Society and Evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (2008)Google Scholar
  27. 27.
    Bühler, K.: Theory of Language: The Representational Function of Language (Donald F. Goodwin, trans.). John Benjamins, Amsterdam (1990 [1934])Google Scholar
  28. 28.
    Sinha, C.: Language and Representation: A Socio-Naturalistic Approach to Human Development. Harvester-Wheatsheaf, Hemel Hempstead (1988)Google Scholar
  29. 29.
    Sinha, C.: The evolution of language: from signals to symbols to system. In: Kimbrough Oller, D., Griebel, U. (eds.) Evolution of Communication Systems: A Comparative Approach. Vienna Series in Theoretical Biology, pp. 217–235. MIT Press, Cambridge (2004)Google Scholar
  30. 30.
    Peirce, C.S.: Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce, vols. 1–6, 1931–1935, eds. Charles Hartshorne and Paul Weiss, vols. 7–8, 1958, ed. Arthur W. Burks. Harvard University Press, Cambridge (1931–1958)Google Scholar
  31. 31.
    Norman, D.: Things that Make us Smart. Addison Wesley, Reading (1993)Google Scholar
  32. 32.
    Donald, M.: Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge (1991)Google Scholar
  33. 33.
    Tomasello, M.: The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge (1999)Google Scholar
  34. 34.
    Bloom, P.: Intention, history, and artifact concepts. Cognition 60, 1–29 (1996)Google Scholar
  35. 35.
    Marx, K.: Capital, vol. 1. Penguin, Harmondsworth (1976 [1867])Google Scholar
  36. 36.
    Tummolini, L., Castelfranchi, C.: The cognitive and behavioral mediation of institutions: towards an account of institutional actions. Cogn. Syst. Res. 7(2–3), 307–323 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. 37.
    Searle, J.: The Construction of Social Reality. Penguin, London (1995)Google Scholar
  38. 38.
    Vygotsky, L.S.: Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Mental Processes. Harvard University Press, Cambridge (1978[1930])Google Scholar
  39. 39.
    Anderson, B.: Imagined Communities. Verso, London (1991)Google Scholar
  40. 40.
    Waddington, C.: Tools for Thought. Paladin, St. Albans (1977)Google Scholar
  41. 41.
    Sinha, C., Rodríguez, C.: Language and the signifying object. In: Zlatev, J., Racine, T., Sinha, C., Itkonen, E. (eds.) The Shared Mind: Perspectives on Intersubjectivity, pp. 358–378. John Benjamins, Amsterdam (2008)Google Scholar
  42. 42.
    Sinha, C.: Objects in a storied world: materiality, narrativity and normativity. J. Conscious. Stud. 16(6–8), 167–190 (2009)Google Scholar
  43. 43.
    Hutchins, E.: Material anchors for conceptual blends. J. Pragmatics 37, 1555–1577 (2005)Google Scholar
  44. 44.
    Sinha, C., Jensen de López, K.: Language, culture and the embodiment of spatial cognition. Cogn. Linguist. 11, 17–41 (2000)Google Scholar
  45. 45.
    Schutz, A.: Collected Papers III Studies in Phenomenological Philosophy. Martinus Nijhoff, The Hague (1966)Google Scholar
  46. 46.
    Lotman, Y.: Universe of the Mind: A Semiotic Theory of Culture (Transl. Ann Shukman). I.B. Tauris and Co. Ltd, New York (1990)Google Scholar
  47. 47.
    Odling-Smee, F.J., Laland, K.N., Feldman, M.W.: Niche Construction: The Neglected Process in Evolution. Princeton University Press, Oxford (2003)Google Scholar
  48. 48.
    Odling-Smee, J., Laland, K.N.: Cultural niche-construction: evolution’s cradle of language. In: Botha, R., Knight, C. (eds.) The Prehistory of Language, pp. 99–121. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. 49.
    Merleau-Ponty, M.: Phenomenology of Perception. Routledge & Kegan Paul, London (1962)Google Scholar
  50. 50.
    Bourdieu, P.: Outline of a Theory of Practice (Transl. R. Nice). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1977)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Languages and LiteratureLund UniversityLundSweden

Personalised recommendations