New Perspectives on Vierordt’s Law: Memory-Mixing in Ordinal Temporal Comparison Tasks

  • Bon-Mi Gu
  • Warren H. Meck
Part of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science book series (LNCS, volume 6789)

Abstract

Distortions in temporal memory can occur as a function of differences in signal modalities and/or by the encoding of multiple signal durations associated with different timing tasks into a single memory distribution – an effect referred to as “memory mixing”. Evidence for this type of memory distortion and/or categorization of signal durations as an explanation for changes in temporal context (e.g., duration ranges), as well as for Vierordt’s law (e.g., overestimation of “short” durations and underestimation of “long” durations), can be studied by examining proactive interference effects from the previous trial(s). Moreover, we demonstrate that individual differences in the magnitude of this “memory-mixing” phenomenon are correlated with variation in reaction times for ordinal temporal comparisons as well as with sensitivity to feedback effects in the formation of duration-specific memory distributions.

Keywords

Timing and time perception Interval timing Memory distributions Individual differences Modality differences Feedback effects Reaction time 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. 1.
    Buhusi, C.V., Meck, W.H.: What Makes us tick? Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 755–765 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Grondin, S.: Overloading Temporal Memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 31, 869–879 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Jazayeri, M., Shadlen, M.N.: Temporal Context Calibrates Interval Timing. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 1020–1026 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Mamassian, P., Landy, M.S.: It’s That Time Again. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 914–916 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Lejeune, H., Wearden, J.H.: Vierordt’s The Experimental Study of the Time Sense and its Legacy. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 21(1868), 941–960 (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Woodrow, H.: Time Perception. In: Stevens, S.S. (ed.) Handbook of Experimental Psychology, pp. 1224–1236. Wiley, New York (1951)Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    Cheng, R.K., Dyke, A.G., McConnell, M.W., Meck, W.H.: Categorical Scaling of Duration as a Function of Temporal Context in Aged Rats. Brain Res. 1381, 175–186 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Cheng, R.K., Scott, A.C., Penney, T.B., Williams, C.L., Meck, W.H.: Prenatal Choline Availability Differentially Modulates Timing of Auditory and Visual Stimuli in Aged Rats. Brain Res. 1237, 167–175 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Penney, T.B.: Modality Differences in Interval Timing: Attention, Clock Speed, and Memory. In: Meck, W.H. (ed.) Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing, pp. 209–234. CRC Press, Boca Raton (2003)Google Scholar
  10. 10.
    Penney, T.B., Allan, L.G., Meck, W.H., Gibbon, J.: Memory Mixing in Duration Bisection. In: Rosenbaum, D.A., Collyer, C.E. (eds.) Timing of Behavior: Neural, Psychological and Computational Perspectives, pp. 165–193. MIT Press, Cambridge (1998)Google Scholar
  11. 11.
    Penney, T.B., Gibbon, J., Meck, W.H.: Differential Effects of Auditory and Visual Signals on Clock Speed and Temporal Memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 26, 1770–1787 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Penney, T.B., Tourret, S.: Modality Effects in Short Interval Timing. Psychologie Française 50, 131–143 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Allan, L.: The Influence of Scalar Timing Model on Human Timing Research. Behav. Processes 44, 101–117 (1998)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Allan, L.G., Gibbon, J.: A New Temporal Illusion or the TOE Once Again? Percept. Psychophys 55, 227–229 (1994)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Brown, G.D.A., McCormack, T., Smith, M., Stewart, N.: Identification and Bisection of Temporal Durations and Tone Frequencies: Common Models for Temporal and Nontemporal Stimuli. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 31, 919–938 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Eisler, H., Eisler, A., Hellström, A.: Psychophysical Issues in the Study of Time Perception. In: Grondin, S. (ed.) Psychology of Time, pp. 75–109. Emerald Group Publishing, Bingley (2008)Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Grondin, S.: Timing and Time Perception: A Review of Recent Behavioral and Neuroscience Findings and Theoretical Directions. Atten. Percept. Psychophys 72, 561–582 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    Hairston, I.S., Nagarajan, S.S.: Neural Mechanisms of the Time-Order Error: An MEG Study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1163–1174 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    Harrington, D.L., Boyd, L.A., Mayer, A.R., Sheltraw, D.M., Lee, R.R., Huang, M., Rao, S.M.: Neural Representation of Interval Encoding and Decision Making. Cogn. Brain Res. 21, 193–205 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Gooch, C.M., Wiener, M., Wencil, E.B., Coslett, H.B.: Interval Timing Disruptions in Subjects with Cerebellar Lesions. Neuropsychologia 48, 1022–1031 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.
    Lustig, C., Matell, M.S., Meck, W.H.: Not “Just” a Coincidence: Frontal-Striatal Synchronization in Working Memory and Interval Timing. Memory 13, 441–448 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. 22.
    Meck, W.H.: Neuropsychology of Timing and Time Perception. Brain Cogn. 58, 1–8 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. 23.
    Meck, W.H.: Neuroantomical Localization of an Internal Clock: A Functional Link Between Mesolimbic, Nigrostriatal, and Mesocortical Dopaminergic Systems. Brain Res. 1109, 93–107 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. 24.
    Meck, W.H., Penney, T.B., Pouthas, V.: Cortico-Striatal Representation of Time in Animals and Humans. Cur. Opin. Neurobiol. 18, 145–152 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. 25.
    Lustig, C., Meck, W.H.: Paying Attention to Time as One Gets Older. Psychol. Sci. 12, 478–484 (2001)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. 26.
    Wearden, J.H., Edwards, H., Fakhri, M., Percival, A.: Why ”Sounds are Judged Longer than Lights”: Application of a Model of the Internal Clock in Humans. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. B. 51, 97–120 (1998)Google Scholar
  27. 27.
    Wearden, J.H., Todd, N.P., Jones, L.A.: When Do Auditory/Visual Differences in Duration Judgments Occur? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 59, 1709–1724 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Meck, W.H., Church, R.M.: Abstraction of Temporal Attributes. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 8, 226–243 (1982)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. 29.
    Gamache, P.L., Grondin, S.: The Lifespan of Time Intervals in Reference Memory. Perception 39, 1431–1451 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. 30.
    Gibbon, J., Church, R.M.: Representation of Time. Cogn. 37, 23–54 (1990)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. 31.
    Gibbon, J., Church, R.M., Meck, W.H.: Scalar Timing in Memory. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 423, 52–77 (1984)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. 32.
    Meck, W.H.: Selective Adjustment of the Speed of Internal Clock and Memory Processes. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 9, 171–201 (1983)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.
    Petrusic, W.M.: Semantic Congruity Effects and Theories of the Comparison Process. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 18, 962–986 (1992)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. 34.
    Petrusic, W.M., Baranski, J.V.: Semantic Congruity Effects in Perceptual Comparisons. Percept. Psychophys. 45, 439–452 (1989)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. 35.
    Chen, Y., Huang, X., Luo, Y., Peng, C., Liu, C.: Differences in the Neural Basis of Automatic Auditory and Visual Time Perception: ERP Evidence From an Across-Modal Delayed Response Oddball Task. Brain Res. 1325, 100–111 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. 36.
    Penney, T.B., Meck, W.H., Roberts, S.A., Gibbon, J., Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L.: Interval-Timing Deficits in Individuals at High Risk for Schizophrenia. Brain Cogn. 58, 109–118 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. 37.
    Gibbon, J., Church, R.M.: Sources of Variance in an Information Processing Theory of Timing. In: Roitblat, H.L., Bever, T.G., Terrace, H.S. (eds.) Animal Cognition, pp. 465–488. Erlbaum, Hillsdale (1984)Google Scholar
  38. 38.
    Lustig, C., Meck, W.H.: Chronic Treatment with Haloperidol Induces Deficits in Working Memory and Feedback Effects of Interval Timing. Brain Cogn. 58, 9–16 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. 39.
    Rakitin, B.C., Malapani, C.: Effects of Feedback on Time Production Errors in Aging Participants. Brain Res. Bul. 75, 22–33 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. 40.
    Adcock, R.A., Thangavel, A., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Knutson, B., Gabrieli, J.D.: Reward-Motivated Learning: Mesolimbic Activation Precedes Memory Formation. Neuron. 50, 507–517 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. 41.
    Koepp, M.J., Gunn, R.N., Lawrence, A.D., Cunningham, V.J., Dagher, A., Jones, T., Brooks, D.J., Bench, C.J., Grasby, P.M.: Evidence For Striatal Dopamine Release During a Video Game. Nature 393, 26–268 (1998)Google Scholar
  42. 42.
    Malapani, C., Rakitin, B., Levy, R., Meck, W.H., Deweer, B., Dubois, B., Gibbon, J.: Coupled Temporal Memories in Parkinson’s Disease: A Dopamine-Related Dysfunction. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 316–331 (1998)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. 43.
    Droit-Volet, S., Meck, W.H., Penney, T.B.: Sensory Modality and Time Perception in Children and Adults. Behav. Processes. 74, 244–250 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. 44.
    Droit-Volet, S., Meck, W.H.: How Emotions Colour Our Perception of Time. Trend Cogn. Sci. 11, 504–513 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. 45.
    Meck, W.H.: Distortions in the Content of Temporal Memory: Neurobiological Correlates. In: Fountain, S.B., Bunsey, M.D., Danks, J.H., McBeath, M.K. (eds.) Animal Cognition and Sequential Behavior: Behavioral, Biological, and Computational Perspectives, pp. 175–200. Kluwer Academic Press, Boston (2002)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. 46.
    Klapproth, F.: Single-Modality Memory Mixing in Temporal Generalization: An Effect Due to Instructional Ambiguity. NeuroQuantology 7, 8–94 (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. 47.
    Vatakis, A., Spence, C.: Temporal Order Judgments for Audiovisual Targets Embedded in Unimodal and Bimodal Distractor Streams. Neurosci. Lett., 5–9 (2006)Google Scholar
  48. 48.
    Allman, M.J., Meck, W.H.: Pathophysiological Distortions in Time Perception and Timed Performance. Brain (in press 2011)Google Scholar
  49. 49.
    Coull, J.T., Cheng, R.K., Meck, W.H.: Neuroanatomical and Neurochemical Substrates of Timing. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 3–25 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. 50.
    Gu, B.-M., Park, J.-Y., Kang, D.-H., Lee, S.J., Yoo, S.Y., Jo, H.J., Choi, C.-H., Lee, J.-M., Kwon, J.S.: Neural Correlates of Cognitive Inflexibility During Task-Switching in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Brain 131, 155–164 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. 51.
    Gu, B.-M., Yin, B., Cheng, R.K., Meck, W.H.: Quinpirole-Induced Sensitization to Noisy/Sparse Periodic Input: Temporal Synchronization as a Component of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Neurosci. 179, 143–150 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Bon-Mi Gu
    • 1
  • Warren H. Meck
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Psychology and NeuroscienceDuke UniversityDurhamUSA

Personalised recommendations