Advertisement

Abstract

During the decades following Piaget’s work, it has been believed that correct judgments of durations require sophisticated reasoning abilities that emerge at about 8 years of age. However, a number of researchers have demonstrated that young children’s poor judgments in classical Piagetian tasks are not due to their inability to judge time correctly, but rather to their limited attentional capacities. Recent research has therefore concentrated on further investigating the development of abilities to discriminate durations in young children on the basis of the temporal tasks initially used with animals within the framework of the internal clock theories. This manuscript reviews and discusses the results of these studies.

Keywords

Child Time Perception Timing Attention 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. 1.
    Bergson, H.: Durée et simultanéité. Presses Universitaires de France (1968)Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Guyau, J.-M.: La genèse de l’idée de temps. Paris, F. Alcan (1890)Google Scholar
  3. 3.
    Piaget, P.: Le développement de la notion de temps chez l’enfant. Paris, P.U.F. (1946)Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Zuili, N., Fraisse, P.: L’estimation du temps en fonction de la quantité de movements effective dans une tâche. Etude génétique. L’Année Psychologique 66, 383–396 (1966)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Fraisse, P., Vautrey, P.: La perception de l’espace, de la vitesse et du temps chez l’enfant de 5 ans. Enfance 5, 102–119 (1952)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Levin, I.: The development of time concepts in young children: Reasoning about duration. Child Development 48, 435–444 (1977)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Levin, I.: Interference of time-related and unrelated cues with duration comparison of young children: Analysis of Piaget’s formulation of the relation of time and speed. Child Development 50, 469–477 (1979)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Arlin, M.: The effect of physical work, mental work, and quantity on children’s time perception. Perception & Psychophysics 45(3), 209–214 (1989)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Berndt, T.J., Wood, D.J.: The development of time concepts through conflict based on a primitive capacity. Child Development 45, 825–828 (1974)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Richie, D.M., Bickard, M.H.: The ability to perceive duration: Its relations to the development of the logical concept of time. Developmental Psychology 24, 318–323 (1988)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Levin, I.: Principles underlying time measurement: The development of children’s constraints on counting time. In: Levin, I., Zakay, D. (eds.) Time and Human Cognition: A Life-span Perspective?, pp. 145–181. Elsevier, Amsterdam (1989)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Friedman, W.: Children’s representations to the daily activities. Child Development 61, 1399–1412 (1990)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Fraisse, P.: Psychologie du temps. Presses Universitaires de France, Paris (1967)Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Pouthas, V., Droit, S., Jacquet, A.Y., Wearden, J.: Differentiation of response duration in children of different ages: Developmental changes in relation between verbal and non verbal behavior? J. Exp. Analysis of Behavior 53, 21–31 (1990)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Droit, S., Pouthas, V., Jacquet, A.Y.: Temporal learning in 4 1/2- and 6-year-old children: Role of instructions and prior knowledge. J. Exp. Child Psychology 50, 305–321 (1990)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Lewis, P., Miall, C.: Remembering the time: A continuous clock. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(9), 401–406 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    Zakay, D.: On prospective time estimation, temporal relevance and temporal uncertainty. In: Macar, F., Pouthas, V., Friedman, W. (eds.) Time, Action and Cognition Toward Bridging the Gap, pp. 141–153. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht (1992)Google Scholar
  18. 18.
    Droit-Volet, S.: What emotions tell us about Time. In: Llyod, D., Arstila, V. (eds.) Subjective Time: The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Temporality. MIT Press, Cambridge (in press) Google Scholar
  19. 19.
    Droit-Volet, S., Gil, S.: The time-emotion paradox. Journal of Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B-Biological Sciences 364, 1943–1953 (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Droit-Volet, S.: Adaptation to time in young children: An initial force rule governing temporal behavior. J. Exp. Child Psychology 68, 236–249 (1998)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.
    Droit-Volet, S., Rattat, A.C.: Are time and action dissociated in young children’s time estimation? Cognitive Development 14, 573–595 (1999)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. 22.
    Rattat, A.C., Droit-Volet, S.: Le transfert d’apprentissage de durée d’action chez le jeune enfant: l’effet facilitateur de la variété des actions? Enfance 54, 141–153 (2002)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. 23.
    Brackbill, Y., Fitzgerald, H.E.: Stereotype temporal conditioning in infants. Psychophysiology 9, 569–577 (1972)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. 24.
    Stamps, L.S.: Temporal conditioning of heart rate responses in newborn infants. Developmental Psychology 13(6), 624–629 (1977)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. 25.
    Colombo, J., Richman, W.A.: Infant timekeeping: Attention and temporal estimation in 4-month-olds. Psychological Science 13, 475–479 (2002)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. 26.
    Brannon, E.M., Libertus, M., Meck, W., Woldorff, M.: Electrophysiological measures of time processing in infant and adult Brains: Weber’s law holds. J. Cognitive Neuroscience 20, 193–203 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. 27.
    Brannon, E.M., Roussel, L.W., Meck, W., Woldorff, M.: Timing in the baby brain. Cognitive Brain Research 21, 227–233 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Pouthas, V.: Adaptation à la durée chez l’enfant de 2 à 5 ans. Année Psy. 81, 33–50 (1981)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. 29.
    Pouthas, V.: Timing behavior in young children: A developmental approach to conditioned spaced responding. In: Michon, J., Jackson, J. (eds.) Time, Mind and Behavior, pp. 100–109. Springer, Berlin (1985)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. 30.
    Bental, R.P., Lowe, C.F.: The role of verbal behavior in human learning: III instructional effects in children. J. Experimental Analysis of Behavior 47, 177–190 (1987)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. 31.
    Bentall, R.P., Lowe, C.F., Beasty, A.: The role of verbal behavior in human learning: II Developmental differences. J. Experimental Analysis of Behavior 43, 165–181 (1985)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. 32.
    Zeiler, M.D., Kelley, C.A.: Fixed-ratio and fixed interval schedules of cartoon presentation. J. Exp. Child Psychology 8, 306–313 (1969)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.
    Long, E.R., Hammack, J.T., May, F., Campbell, B.J.: Intermittent reinforcement of operant behavior in children. J. Exp. Analysis of Behavior 1, 315–339 (1958)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. 34.
    Dempster, F.N., Brainerd, C.J.: Interference and inhibition in cognition. Academic Press, New York (1995)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. 35.
    Pouthas, V., Jacquet, A.Y.: A developmental study of timing behavior in 4 1/2- and 7-year-old children. J. Exp. Child Psychology 43, 282–299 (1987)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. 36.
    Droit, S.: Temporal regulation of behavior with an external clock in 3-year-old children: differences between waiting and response duration tasks. J. Exp. Child Psy. 58, 332–345 (1994)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. 37.
    Lowe, C.F., Beasty, A., Bentall, R.P.: The role of verbal behavior in human learning: Infant performance on Fixed-Interval Schedules. J. Exp. Analysis of Behavior 39, 157–164 (1983)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. 38.
    Darcheville, J.C., Rivière, V., Wearden, J.H.: Fixed-interval performance and self-control in children. J. Experimental Analysis of Behavior 57, 187–199 (1992)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. 39.
    Darcheville, J.C., Rivière, V., Wearden, J.H.: Fixed-interval performance and self-control in infants. J. Experimental Analysis of Behavior 60, 239–254 (1993)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. 40.
    Clément, C., Lattal, K.A., Rivière, V., Darcheville, J.-C.: Understanding the ontogenesis of temporal regulation in infants and children: An evaluation of the procedures. European Journal of Behavior Analysis 8, 41–48 (2007)Google Scholar
  41. 41.
    Droit-Volet, S.: Scalar timing in temporal generalization in children with short and long stimulus durations. The Quarterly J. Exp Psy. 55A, 1193–1209 (2002)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. 42.
    Droit-Volet, S., Clément, A., Wearden, J.: Temporal Generalization in children. J. Exp. Child Psy. 80, 271–288 (2001)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. 43.
    McCormack, T., Brown, G.D.A., Maylor, E.A., Darby, A., Green, D.: Developmental changes in time estimation: Comparing childhood and old age. Develop. Psy. 35, 1143–1155 (1999)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. 44.
    Droit-Volet, S., Wearden, J.H.: Temporal bisection in children. J. Exp. Child Psy. 80, 142–159 (2001)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. 45.
    Kopec, C.D., Brody, S.D.: Human performance on the temporal bisection task. Brain and Cognition 74, 262–272 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. 46.
    Church, R.M., Deluty, M.Z.: Bisection of temporal intervals. J. Exp. Psy. 3, 216–228 (1977)Google Scholar
  47. 47.
    Allan, L.G., Gibbon, J.: Human bisection at the geometric mean. Learning and Motivation 22, 39–58 (1991)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. 48.
    Wearden, J.H.: Human performance on an analogue of an interval bisection task. The Quarterly J. Exp. Psychology 43, 59–81 (1991)Google Scholar
  49. 49.
    Melgire, M., Ragot, R., Samson, S., Penney, T.B., Meck, W., Pouthas, V.: Auditory/visual duration bisection in patients with left or right medial-temporal lobe resection. Brain and Cognition 58, 119–124 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. 50.
    Penney, T., Gibbon, J., Meck, W.: Differential effects of auditory and visual signals on clock speed and temporal memory. J. Exp. Psy.: H.P.P. 26, 1770–1787 (2000)Google Scholar
  51. 51.
    Allan, L.G.: Are the referents remembered in temporal bisection? Learning and Motivation 33, 10–31 (2002)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. 52.
    Smith, J.G., Harper, D.N., Gittings, D., Abernethy, D.: The effect of Parkinson’s disease on time estimation as a function of stimulus duration range and modality. Brain and Cognition 64, 130–143 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. 53.
    Wearden, J.H., Todd, N.P.M., Jones, L.A.: When do auditory/visual differences in duration judgment occur? The Quarterly J. Exp. Psychology 59, 1709–1724 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. 54.
    Provasi, J., Rattat, A.C., Droit-Volet, S.: Temporal bisection in 4-month-old infants. J. Exp. Psy.: Animal Behavior Processes 37(1), 108–113 (2010)Google Scholar
  55. 55.
    VanMarle, K., Wynn, K.: Six-month-olds infants use analog magnitudes to represent durations. Developmental Science 9, 41–49 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. 56.
    Brannon, E.M., Suanda, S., Libertus, K.: Temporal discrimination increases in precision over development and parallels the development of numerosity discrimination. Developmental Science 10(6), 770–777 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. 57.
    Gibbon, J.: Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychological Review 84, 279–325 (1977)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. 58.
    Gibbon, J., Church, R.M., Meck, W.H.: Scalar timing in memory. In: Gibbon, J., Allan, L. (eds.) Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423: Timing and Time Perception, pp. 52–77. New York Academy of Sciences, New York (1984)Google Scholar
  59. 59.
    Gibbon, J.: On the form and location of the bisection function for time. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 24, 58–87 (1981)zbMATHCrossRefMathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  60. 60.
    Droit-Volet, S., Clément, A., Fayol, M.: Time, number and length: Similarities and differences in bisection behavior in children and adults. The Q. J. Exp. Psy. 61(12), 1827–1846 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. 61.
    Meck, W.H.: Affinity for the dopamine D2 receptor predicts neuroleptic potency in decreasing the speed of an internal clock. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 25, 1185–1189 (1986)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. 62.
    Meck, W.H.: Selective adjustment of speed of internal clock and memory processes. J. Exp. Psy.: Animal Behavior Processes 9, 171–201 (1983)Google Scholar
  63. 63.
    Droit-Volet, S., Izaute, M.: The effect of feedback on timing in children and adults: the temporal generalization task. The Quarterly J. Exp. Psy. 58(3), 507–520 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. 64.
    Droit-Volet, S., Izaute, M.: Improving time discrimination in children and adults in a temporal bisection task: The effects of feedback and no-forced choice on decision and memory processes. The Quarterly J. Exp. Psy. 62(6), 1173–1188 (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. 65.
    Wearden, J.H., Ferrara, A.: Stimulus spacing effects in temporal bisection by humans. The Quarterly J. Exp. Psycholgy 48B(4), 289–310 (1995)Google Scholar
  66. 66.
    Wearden, J.H., Ferrara, A.: Stimulus range effects in temporal bisection by humans. The Quarterly J. Exp. Psychology 49B(1), 24–44 (1996)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. 67.
    Droit-Volet, S., Tourret, S., Wearden, J.H.: Perception of the duration of auditory and visual stimuli in children and adults. Quarterly J. Exp. Psychology 57, 797–818 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. 68.
    Zélanti, P., Droit-Volet, S.: Cognitive abilities explaining age-related changes in time perception of short and long durations. J. Exp. Child Psychology 109(2), 143–157 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. 69.
    Droit-Volet, S., Clément, A.: Time perception in children and adults: Effects of continuous and discontinuous signal. Current Psychology of Cognition 23(3), 229–248 (2005)Google Scholar
  70. 70.
    Droit-Volet, S., Rattat, A.C.: A further analysis of temporal bisection behavior in children with and without reference memory: The similiarity and the partition task. Acta Psychologica 125, 240–256 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. 71.
    Rattat, A.C., Droit-Volet, S.: Variability in 5- and 8-year-olds’ memory for duration: An interfering task in temporal bisection. Behavioural Processes 55, 81–91 (2001)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. 72.
    Wearden, J.H., Lejeune, H.: Scalar properties in human timing: Conformity and violations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 61, 569–587 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. 73.
    Treisman, M.: Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the “internal clock”. Psychological Monographs 77, 1–13 (1963)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. 74.
    Livesey, A.C., Wall, M.B., Smith, A.T.: Time perception: Manipulation of task difficulty dissociates clock functions for other demands. Neuropsychologia 45, 321–331 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. 75.
    Eagleman, D.M.: News & Views: Distortions of time during rapid eye movements. Nature Neuroscience 87, 850–851 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. 76.
    Coull, J.T., Cheng, R.K., Meck, W.H.: Neuroanatomical and neurochemical substrates of timing. Neuropsychopharmacology 36(1), 3–25 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. 77.
    Matell, M.S., Meck, W.H.: Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. Bioessays 22, 94–103 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. 78.
    Matell, M.S., Meck, W.H.: Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: Coincidence-detection of oscillatory processes. Cognitive Brain Research 21, 139–170 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. 79.
    Karmarkar, U.R., Buonomano, D.V.: Timing in the absence of clocks: Encoding time in neural network states. Neuron 53, 427–438 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. 80.
    Mauk, M., Buonomano, D.: The neural basis of temporal processing. Annual Review of Neurosciences 27, 207–340 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. 81.
    Tsujimoto, S.: The Prefrontal Cortex: Functional Neural Development During Early Childhood. The Neuroscientist 14(4), 345–358 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. 82.
    Casey, B.J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., Durston, S.: Imaging the developing brain: What have we learned about cognitive development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9, 104–110 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. 83.
    Sowell, E.R., Thompson, P.M., Holmes, C.J., Batth, R., Jernigan, T.L., Toga, A.W.: Localizing Age-Related Changes in Brain Structure between Childhood and Adolescence Using Statistical Parametric Mapping. NeuroImage 9, 587–597 (1999)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. 84.
    Maricq, A.V., Roberts, S., Church, R.M.: Metamphetamine and time estimation. J. Exp. Psy.: Animal Behavior Processes 7, 18–30 (1981)Google Scholar
  85. 85.
    Rammsayer, T.: Is there a common dopaminergic basis of time perception and reaction time? Neuropsychobiology 21, 37–42 (1989)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. 86.
    Rammsayer, T.: Effects of pharmacologically induced dopamine-receptor stimulation on human temporal information processing. NeuroQuantology 7, 103–113 (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. 87.
    Kienast, T., et al.: Dopamine in amygdala gates limbic processing of aversive stimuli in humans. Nature Neuroscience 11, 1381–1382 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. 88.
    Bar-Haim, Y., Kerem, A., Lamy, D., Zakay, D.: When time slow down: The influence of threat on time perception in anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 1–9 (2009)Google Scholar
  89. 89.
    Droit-Volet, S., Brunot, S., Niedenthal, P.M.: Perception of the duration of emotional events. Cognition and Emotion 18, 849–858 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. 90.
    Droit-Volet, S., Mermillod, M., Cocenas-Silva, R., Gil, S.: The effect of expectancy of a threatening event on time perception in human adults. Emotion 10(6), 908–914 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. 91.
    Langer, J., Wapner, S., Werner, H.: The effect of danger upon the experience of time. American Journal of Psychology 74, 94–97 (1966)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. 92.
    Watts, F.N., Sharrock, R.: Fear and time estimation. P. & Motor Skills 59, 597–598 (1984)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. 93.
    Gil, S., Niedenthal, P., Droit-Volet, S.: Anger and temporal perception in children. Emotion 7, 219–225 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. 94.
    Droit-Volet, S., Meck, W.H.: How emotions colour our time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 1(12), 504–513 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. 95.
    Gil, S., Droit-Volet, S.: How do emotional facial expressions influence our perception of time? In: Masmoudi, S., Yan Dai, D., Naceur, A. (eds.) Attention, Representation, and Human Performance: Integration of Cognition, Emotion and Motivation. Psychology Press, Taylor & Francis, London (2011)Google Scholar
  96. 96.
    Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., Naish, P.L.N., Brogan, D.: The internal clock: Evidence for a temporal oscillator underlying time perception with some estimates of its characteristic frequency. Perception 19, 705–743 (1990)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. 97.
    Wearden, J.H., Smith-Spark, J.H., Cousins, R., Edelstyn, N.M.J., Cody, F.W.J., O’Boyle, D.J.: Effect of click trains on duration estimated by people with Parkinson’s disease. The Quarterly J. Exp. Psychology 62, 33–40 (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. 98.
    Droit-Volet, S., Wearden, J.H.: Speeding up an internal clock in children? Effects of visual flicker on subjective duration. Quarterly J. Exp. Psychology 55, 193–211 (2002)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. 99.
    Droit-Volet, S.: Speeding up a master clock common to time, number and length? Behavioural Processes 85, 126–134 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. 100.
    Jones, L.A., Allely, C.A., Wearden, J.H.: Click trains and the rate of information processing: Does “speeding up” subjective time make other psychological processes run faster? The Quarterly J. Exp. Psy. 64, 363–380 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. 101.
    Rattat, A., Droit-Volet, S.: Development of long-term memory for duration in a temporal bisection task. The Quarterly J. Exp. Psy. 58B(2), 163–176 (2005)Google Scholar
  102. 102.
    Rattat, A.C., Droit-Volet, S.: Implicit long-term memory for duration in young children. The European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 19, 271–285 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. 103.
    Delgado, M.L., Droit-Volet, S.: Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection and the generalization task: The utility of a developmental approach. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 60(6), 820–836 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. 104.
    Meaux, J.B., Chelonis, J.J.: Time perception differences in children with and without ADHD. J. Pediatr. Health Care 17(2), 64–71 (2003)Google Scholar
  105. 105.
    Smith, A., Taylor, E., Warner Rogers, J., Newman, S., Rubia, K.: Evidence for a pure time perception deficit in children with ADHD. J. Child Psy. and Psychiatry 43(4), 529–542 (2002)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. 106.
    Toplak, M.E., Dockstader, C., Tannock, R.: Traitement de l’information temporelle dans le TDAH: les résultats à ce jour et de nouvelles méthodes. Journal of Neurosciences Methods 151, 15–29 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. 107.
    Cohen, M.J.: Examiner’s manual: Children’s memory scale (1997)Google Scholar
  108. 108.
    Baddeley, A.D., Hitch, G.: Working memory. In: Bower, G. (ed.) The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, pp. 47–90. Academic Press, New York (1974)Google Scholar
  109. 109.
    Baddeley, A.D., Logie, R.H.: The multiple-component model. In: Miyake, A., Shah, P. (eds.) Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control, pp. 28–61. Cambridge University Press, New York (1999)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. 110.
    Baudouin, A., Vanneste, S., Pouthas, V., Isingrini, M.: Age-related changes in duration reproduction: Involvement of working memory processes. Brain and Cognition 62, 17–23 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. 111.
    Fink, A., Neubauer, A.C.: Individual differences in time estimation related to cognitive ability, speed of information processing and working memory. Intelligence 33, 5–26 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. 112.
    Ulbrich, P., Churan, J., Fink, M., Wittman, M.: Temporal reproduction: Further evidence for 2 processes. Acta Psychologica 125, 51–65 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  113. 113.
    Lejeune, H.: Switching or gating? The attentional challenge in cognitive models of psychological time. Behavioural Processes 44, 127–145 (1998)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. 114.
    Ivry, R.B., Schlerf, J.E.: Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12, 273–280 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. 115.
    Droit-Volet, S.: Alerting attention and time perception in children. J. Exp. Child Psychology 85(4), 372–394 (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. 116.
    Brown, S.: Attentional resources in timing: Interference effects in concurrent temporal and non-temporal working memory tasks. Perception & Psychophysics 59, 1118–1140 (1997)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  117. 117.
    Fortin, C., Rousseau, R.: Interference from short-term memory processing on encoding and reproducting brief durations. Psychological Research 61, 269–276 (1998)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  118. 118.
    Macar, F., Grondin, S., Casini, L.: Controlled attention sharing influences time estimation. Memory and Cognition 22, 673–686 (1994)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  119. 119.
    Arlin, M.: The effects of quantity and depth of processing on children’s time perception. J. of Experimental Child Psychology 42, 84–98 (1986)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. 120.
    Gautier, T., Droit-Volet, S.: Attention and time estimation in 5- and 8-year-old children: A dual-task procedure. Behavioural Processes 58, 56–66 (2002)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  121. 121.
    Gautier, T., Droit-Volet, S.: The impact of attentional distraction on temporal bisection in children. International Journal of Psychology 37, 27–34 (2002)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  122. 122.
    Zakay, D.: The role of attention in children’s time perception. J. Exp. Child Psy. 54, 355–371 (1992)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  123. 123.
    Droit-Volet, S., Meck, W., Penney, T.: Sensory modality effect and time perception in children and adults. Behavioural Processes 74, 244–250 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  124. 124.
    Szelag, E., Kowalska, J., Rymarczyk, K., Pöppel, E.: Duration processing in children as determined by time reproduction: implications for a few seconds temporal window. Acta Psychologica 110, 1–19 (2002)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. 125.
    Droit-Volet, S.: A further investigation of the filled duration illusion with the comparison between children and adults. J. Exp. Psy.: Animal Behavior Processes 34, 400–414 (2008)Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Sylvie Droit-Volet
    • 1
  1. 1.Laboratoire de Psychologie Sociale & Cognitive, CNRS (UMR 6024)Université Blaise PascalClermont-FerrandFrance

Personalised recommendations