Coordination of Group Movements in Non-human Primates

  • Claudia Fichtel
  • Lennart Pyritz
  • Peter M. Kappeler
Chapter

Abstract

Many animals are organised into social groups. Because individuals have different preferences and diverging needs, conflicts of interests exist; these conflicts are particularly revealed and negotiated in the context of group movements. Thus, group movements provide an excellent example to study coordination processes in non-human primates. In this chapter we review several aspects related to group movements in non-human primates. We first summarise the current understanding of variation in spacing patterns, types of leadership, and decision-making processes. We then focus on methodological issues and discuss various operational definitions of group movements, and we propose an operational definition that has already been applied successfully in studies of small free-ranging groups. We conclude by discussing the possibilities and limitations of transferring concepts and methods from studies of non-human primate groups to research on human groups.

References

  1. Alberts SC, Altmann J, Wilson ML (1996) Mate guarding constrains foraging activity of male baboons. Anim Behav 51:1269–1277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Alexander RD (1974) The evolution of social behavior. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 5:325–383CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Alexander RD (1987) The biology of moral systems. Aldine, LondonGoogle Scholar
  4. Altmann J (1980) Baboon mothers and infants. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  5. Altmann SA (1979) Baboon progressions: order or chaos? A study of one dimensional group geometry. Anim Behav 27:46–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Beauchamp G (2000) Individual differences in activity and exploration influence leadership in pairs of foraging zebra finches. Behaviour 137:301–314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Benadi G, Fichtel C, Kappeler P (2008) Intergroup relations and home range use in Verreaux’s sifaka (Propithecus verreauxi). Am J Primatol 70:956–965Google Scholar
  8. Bertram BCR (1978) Living in groups: predators and prey. In: Krebs JR, Davies JB (eds) Behavioural ecology, 3rd edn. Blackwell Scientific, Oxford, pp 64–96Google Scholar
  9. Boesch C (1991a) The effects of leopard predation on grouping patterns in forest chimpanzees. Behaviour 117:220–242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Boesch C (1991b) Symbolic communication in wild chimpanzees? Hum Evol 6:81–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Boinski S (1988) Sex differences in the foraging behavior of squirrel monkeys in a seasonal habitat. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 23:177–186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Boinski S (1991) The coordination of spatial position: a field study of the vocal behavior of adult female squirrel monkeys. Anim Behav 41:89–102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Boinski S (1993) Vocal coordination of group movement among white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus. Am J Primatol 30:85–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Boinski S (2000) Social manipulation within and between troops mediates primate group movement. In: Boinski S, Garber PA (eds) On the move: how and why animals travel in groups. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 421–469Google Scholar
  15. Boinski S, Campbell AF (1995) Use of trill vocalisations to coordinate troop movement among white-faced capuchins: a second field test. Behaviour 132:875–901CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Boinski S, Cropp S (1999) Disparate data sets resolve squirrel monkey (Saimiri) taxonomy: Implications for behavioral ecology and biomedical usage. Int J Primatol 20:237–256CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Boinski S, Garber PA (eds) (2000) On the move: How and why animals travel in groups. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  18. Boinski S, Moraes E, Kleiman D, Dietz J, Baker A (1994) Intra-group vocal behavior in wild golden lion tamarins, Leontopithecus rosalia: Honest communication of individual activity. Behaviour 130:53–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Byrne RW (1981) Distance vocalization of Guinea baboons (Papio papio): an analysis of function. Behaviour 78:283–312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Byrne RW (2000) How monkeys find their way: leadership, coordination, and cognitive maps of African baboons. In: Boinski S, Garber P (eds) On the move. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 491–518Google Scholar
  21. Chapman CA (1990) Association patterns of spider monkeys: the influence of ecology and sex on social organization. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 26:409–414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Chapman CA, Wrangham R, Chapman L (1995) Ecological constraints on group size: an analysis of spider monkey and chimpanzee subgroups. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 36:59–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Cheney DL (1987) Interactions and relationships between groups. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 267–281Google Scholar
  24. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2007) Baboon metaphysics: the evolution of a social mind. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  25. Conradt L, Roper TJ (2003) Group decision-making in animals. Nature 421:155–158PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Conradt L, Roper TJ (2005) Consensus decision making in animals. Trends Ecol Evol 20:449–456PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Conradt L, Roper TJ (2007) Democracy in animals: the evolution of shared group decisions. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 274:2317–2326CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Cornelius C, Boos M (2003) Enhancing mutual understanding in synchronous computer-mediated communication by training: trade-offs in judgmental tasks. Commun Res 30:147–177CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Couzin ID, Krause J, James R, Ruxton GD, Franks NR (2002) Collective memory and spatial sorting in animal groups. J Theoret Biol 218:1–11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Couzin ID, Krause J, Franks NR, Levin SA (2005) Effective leadership and decision-making in animal groups on the move. Nature 43:513–516CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Dunbar RIM, Dunbar EP (1975) Social dynamics of gelada baboons. Contrib Primatol 6:1–157PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Dunbar RIM, Dunbar EP (1988) Maternal time budgets of gelada baboons. Anim Behav 36:970–980CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Dunbar RIM, Shultz S (2007) Evolution in the social brain. Science 317:1344–1347PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Dyer JRG, Johansson A, Helbing D, Couzin ID, Krause J (2009) Leadership, consensus decision making and collective behavior in humans. Phil Trans Roy Soc Lond B 364:781–789CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Eisenberg JF (1981) The mammalian radiations. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  36. Erhart EM, Overdorff DJ (1998) Group leadership and feeding priority in wild Propithecus diadema edwardsi and Eulemur fulvus rufus. Am J Primatol 45:178–179Google Scholar
  37. Erhart EM, Overdorff DJ (1999) Female coordination of group travel in wild Propithecus and Eulemur. Int J Primatol 20:927–941CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Fischhoff IR, Sundaresan SR, Cordingley J, Larkin HM, Sellier M-J, Rubenstein DI (2007) Social relationships and reproductive state influence leadership roles in movements of plains zebra, Equus burchellii. Anim Behav 73:825–831CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Goodall J (1968) The behavior of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Anim Behav Monogr 1:161–311Google Scholar
  40. Harcourt J, Ang T, Sweetman G, Johnstone R, Manica A (2009) Social feedback and the emergence of leaders and followers. Curr Biol 19:248–252PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Hemelrijk CK (2002) Understanding social behavior with the help of complexity science. Ethology 108:655–671CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Ingmanson EJ (1996) Tool using behavior in wild Pan paniscus: social and ecological considerations. In: Russon AE, Bard K, Taylor S (eds) Reaching into thought: the minds of the great apes. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 190–210Google Scholar
  43. Itani J (1963) Vocal communication of the wild Japanese monkeys. Primates 4:11–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Jacobs A, Maumy M, Petit O (2008) The influence of social organisation on leadership in brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus fulvus) in a controlled environment. Behav Process 79:111–113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Jouventin P (1975) Observations sur la socioecologie du mandrill [in French]. Terre et Vie 29:493–532Google Scholar
  46. Kappeler PM (2000) Grouping and movement patterns in Malagasy primates. In: Boinski S, Garber PA (eds) On the move: how and why animals travel in groups. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 470–490Google Scholar
  47. Kappeler P, Port M (2008) Mutual tolerance or reproductive competition? Patterns of reproductive skew among male redfronted lemurs (Eulemur fulvus rufus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:1477–1488CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Kappeler PM, Schäffler L (2008) The lemur syndrome unresolved: Extreme male reproductive sew in sifakas (Propithecus verreauxi), a sexually monomorphic primate with female dominance. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:1007–1015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Kerth G, Ebert C, Schmidtke C (2006) Group decision making in fission-fusion societies: evidence from two field-experiments in Bechstein’s bats. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 273:2785–2790CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. King AJ, Douglas CMS, Huchard E, Isaac NJB, Cowlishaw G (2008) Dominance and affiliation mediate despotism in a social primate. Curr Biol 18:1833–1838PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Krause J, Reeves P, Hoare D (1998) Positioning behavior in roach shoals: the role of body length and nutritional state. Behaviour 135:1031–1039CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Kudo H (1987) The study of vocal communication of wild mandrills in Cameroon in relation to their social structure. Primates 28:289–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Kummer H (1968) Social organisation of hamadryas baboons. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  54. Kummer H (1995) In quest of the sacred baboon: a scientist’s journey. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJGoogle Scholar
  55. Leblond C, Reebs SG (2006) Individual leadership and boldness in shoals of golden shiners (Notemigonus crysoleucas). Behaviour 143:1263–1280CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Leca JB, Gunst N, Thierry B, Petit O (2003) Distributed leadership in semi-free ranging white-faced capuchin monkeys. Anim Behav 66:1045–1052CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Marler P (1965) Communication in monkeys and apes. In: DeVore I (ed) Primate behavior. Holt Rinehart Winston, New York, pp 544–584Google Scholar
  58. Maynard Smith J, Szathmáry E (1995) The major transitions in evolution. WH Freeman/Spektrum, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  59. Mehlman PT (1996) Branch shaking and related displays in wild Barbary macaques. In: Fa JE, Lindburg DG (eds) Evolution and ecology of macaque societies. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 503–526Google Scholar
  60. Menzel CR (1993) Coordination and conflict in Callicebus social groups. In: Mason WA, Mendoza SP (eds) Primate social conflict. State University of New York Press, Albany, pp 253–290Google Scholar
  61. Meunier H, Leca JB, Deneubourg JL, Petit O (2006) Group movement decisions in capuchin monkeys: the utility of an experimental study and a mathematical model to explore the relationship between individual and collective behaviors. Behaviour 143:1511–1527CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Milton K (1980) The foraging strategy of howler monkeys: a study in primate economics. Columbia University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  63. Mitchell CL, Boinski S, van Schaik CP (1991) Competitive regimes and females bonding in two species of squirrel monkey (Saimiri oerstedi and S. sciureus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 28:55–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Neville MK (1968) Ecology and activity of Himalayan foothill rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Ecology 49:110–123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Norton GW (1986) Leadership decision processes of group movement in yellow baboons. In: Else JG, Lee PC (eds) Primate ecology and conservation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 145–156Google Scholar
  66. Noser R, Byrne RW (2007a) Travel routes and planning of visits to out-of-sight resources in wild chacma baboons, Papio ursinus. Anim Behav 73:257–266CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Noser R, Byrne RW (2007b) Mental maps in chacma baboons (Papio urisnus): Using inter-group encounters as a natural experiment. Anim Cogn 10:331–340PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Oates JF (1977) The guereza and its food. In: Clutton-Brock TH (ed) Primate ecology. Academic, London, pp 276–321Google Scholar
  69. Ostner J, Kappeler PM (1999) Central males instead of multiple pairs in redfronted lemurs, Eulemur fulvus rufus (Primates, Lemuridae)? Anim Behav 58:1069–1078PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Overdorff DJ (1993) Similarities, differences, and seasonal patterns in diets of Eulemur rubriventer and Eulemur fulvus rufus in the Ranomafana National Park, Madagascar. Int J Primatol 14:721–753CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Overdorff DJ (1996) Ecological correlates to activity and habitat use of two prosimian primates: Eulemur rubriventer and Eulemur fulvus rufus in Madagascar. Am J Primatol 40:327–342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Parrish JK, Edelstein-Keshet L (1999) Complexity, pattern, and evolutionary trade-offs in animal aggregation. Science 284:99–101PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Piyapong C, Morrell LJ, Croft DP, Dyer JRG, Ioannou CC, Krause J (2007) A cost of leadership in human groups. Ethology 113:821–824CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Pollock IJ (1997) Female dominance in Indri indri. Folia Primatol 31:143–164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Pulliam HR, Caraco T (1984) Living in groups: is there an optimal group size? In: Krebs JR, Davies MNO (eds) Behavioural ecology. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 122–147Google Scholar
  76. Pyritz L, Fichtel C, Kappeler P (2010) Conceptual and methodological issues in the comparative study of collective group movements. Behav Process 84:681–684Google Scholar
  77. Rands SA, Cowlishaw G, Pettifor RA, Rowcliffe JM, Johnstone RA (2003) Spontaneous emergence of leaders and followers in foraging pairs. Nature 423:432–434PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Rands SA, Cowlishaw G, Pettifor RA, Rowcliffe JM, Johnstone RA (2008) The emergence of leaders and followers in foraging pairs when the qualities of individuals differ. BMC Evol Biol 8:51PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Reader SM, Laland KN (2002) Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99:4436–4441PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Reebs SG (2000) Can a minority of informed leaders determine the foraging movements of a fish shoal? Anim Behav 59:403–409PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Reebs SG (2001) Influence of body size on leadership in shoals of Golden Shiners, Notemigonus crysoleucas. Behaviour 138:797–809CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Rowell TE (1969) Intra-sexual behavior and female reproductive cycles of baboons (Papio anubis). Anim Behav 17:159–167PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Rowell TE (1972) Female reproductive cycles and social behavior in primates. Adv Stud Behav 4:69–105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Sauther ML, Sussman RW (1993) A new interpretation of the social organization and mating system of the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta). In: Kappeler PM, Ganzhorn JU (eds) Lemur social systems and their ecological basis. Plenum, New York, pp 111–121Google Scholar
  85. Schaller G (1963) The mountain gorilla: Ecology and behavior. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  86. Scholz F, Kappeler PM (2004) Effects of seasonal water scarcity on the ranging behavior of Eulemur fulvus rufus. Int J Primatol 25:599–613CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Schulz-Hardt S, Brodbeck FC, Mojzisch A, Kerschreiter R, Frey D (2006) Group decision making in hidden profile situations: dissent as a facilitator for decision quality. J Pers Soc Psychol 91:1080–1091PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Seeley TD, Visscher PK (2004) Quorum sensing during nest-site selection by honeybe swarms. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 56:594–601CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Seyfarth RM (1986) Vocal communication and its relation to language. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. Chicago University Press, Chicago, pp 440–451Google Scholar
  90. Sigg H, Stolba A (1981) Home range and daily march in a hamadryas baboon troop. Folia Primatol 36:40–75PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J (2003) Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science 302:1231–1234PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) (1986) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  93. Soini P (1981) The pygmy marmoset, genus Cebuella. In: Mittermeier RA, Rylands AB, Coimbra-Filho AF (eds) Ecology and behavior of Neotropical primates. Academia Brasileira de Ciencias, Rio de Janeiro, pp 79–129Google Scholar
  94. Stanford CB (1990) Colobine socioecology and female-bonded models of primate social structure. Kroeber Anthropol Soc Pap 71–72:21–28Google Scholar
  95. Sterck EHM, Watts DP, van Schaik CP (1997) The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 41:291–309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Stewart KJ, Harcourt AH (1994) Gorillas` vocalisations during rest periods: signals of impending departure? Behaviour 130:29–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Struhsaker TT (1967a) Auditory communication among vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). In: Altmann SA (ed) Social communication among primates. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 281–324Google Scholar
  98. Struhsaker TT (1967b) Ecology of vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops) in the Masai-Amboseli Game Reserve, Kenya. Ecology 48:891–904CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Struhsaker TT (1975) The red colobus monkey. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  100. Stueckle S, Zinner D (2008) To follow or not to follow: decision making and leadership during the morning departure in chacma baboons. Anim Behav 75:1995–2004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Sueur C, Petit O (2008a) Shared or unshared consensus decision in macaques? Behav Process 78:84–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Sueur C, Petit O (2008b) Organization of group members at departure is driven by social structure in Macaca. Int J Primatol 29:1085–1098CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Trillmich J, Fichtel C, Kappeler PM (2004) Coordination of group movements in wild Verreux's sifakas (Propithecus verreauxi). Behaviour 141:1103–1120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. van Nordwijk MA, van Schaik CP (1987) Competition among female long-tailed macaques, Macaca fascicularis. Anim Behav 35:577–589CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. van Nordwijk MA, Hemelrijk C, Herremans L, Sterck E (1993) Spatial position and behavioral sex differences in juvenile long-tailed macaques. In: Pereira M, Fairbanks L (eds) Juvenile primates. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 77–85Google Scholar
  106. van Schaik CP (1983) Why are diurnal primates living in groups? Behaviour 87:120–144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. van Schaik CP (1989) The ecology of social relationships amongst female primates. In: Standen V, Foley RA (eds) Comparative socioecology. The behavioral ecology of humans and other mammals. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 195–218Google Scholar
  108. van Vugt M (2006) Evolutionary origins of leadership and followership. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 10:354–371PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Vogel C (1973) Acoustical communication among free-ranging common Indian langurs (Presbytis entellus) in two different habitats of North India. Am J Phys Anthropol 38:469–480PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. Ward AJW, Thomas P, Hart PJB, Krause J (2004) Correlates of boldness in three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:561–568CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Watts DP (1994) The influence of male mating tactics on habitat use in mountain gorillas (Gorilla gorilla berengei). Primates 35:35–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. Whitehead JM (1989) The effect of the location of a simulated intruder on responses to long-distance vocalizations of mantled howling monkeys, Alouatta palliata palliata. Anim Behav 108:73–103Google Scholar
  113. Wrangham RW (2000) Why are male chimpanzees more gregarious than mothers? A scramble competition hypothesis. In: Kappeler PM (ed) Primate males. Causes and consequences of variation in group composition. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 248–258Google Scholar
  114. Zeller AC (1986) Communication by sight and smell. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 433–439Google Scholar
  115. Zemel A, Lubin Y (1995) Inter-group competition and stable group sizes. Anim Behav 50:485–488CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Claudia Fichtel
    • 1
  • Lennart Pyritz
    • 1
  • Peter M. Kappeler
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Behavioural Ecology and SociobiologyGerman Primate CenterGöttingenGermany

Personalised recommendations