Skip to main content

Planung von Handlungssequenzen

  • Chapter
  • 5277 Accesses

Part of the Springer-Lehrbuch book series (SLB)

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • DOI: 10.1007/978-3-642-12858-5_7
  • Chapter length: 16 pages
  • Instant PDF download
  • Readable on all devices
  • Own it forever
  • Exclusive offer for individuals only
  • Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout
eBook
USD   29.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • ISBN: 978-3-642-12858-5
  • Instant PDF download
  • Readable on all devices
  • Own it forever
  • Exclusive offer for individuals only
  • Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Weiterführende Literatur

  • Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L.A. Jeffress (ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior(pp. 112–146). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. & Norman, D. A. (1982). Simulating a skilled typist: A study of skilled cognitive-motor performance. Cognitive Science, 6, 1–36.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

Literatur

  • Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283–321.

    PubMed  CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S., Burger, L. K. & Svec, W. R. (1997). Language production and serial order: A functional analysis and a model. Psychological Review, 104, 123–147.

    PubMed  CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. W., Pollack, M. D. & Montague, W. E. (1970). Implicit speech: Mechanism in perceptual encoding? Journal of Experimental Psychology, 84, 502–507.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K. (1972). An associative basis for coding and organization in memory. In A. W. Melton & E. Martin (eds.), Coding processes in human memory(pp. 161–190). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fromkin, V. A. (ed.). (1980). Errors in linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen, and hand. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garcia-Colera, A. & Semjen, A. (1988). Distributed planning of movement sequences. Journal of Motor Behavior, 20, 341– 367.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, M. F. (1982). Production of speech: Observations from normal and pathological language use. In A. W. Ellis (ed.), Normality and pathology in cognitive functions(pp. 19–76). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D. R. (1983). The acquisition of typewriting skill. Acta Psychologica, 54, 233–248.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D. R., Grudin, J. & Conway, E. (1980). Finger movements in transciption typing. La Jolla, CA: University of California, San Diego, Center for Human Information Processing (Technical Report 8001).

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G. & Simon, H. A. (1974). Processes for sequence production. Psychological Review, 81, 187–198.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideomotor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.

    PubMed  CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, D. L. & Haaland, K. Y. (1987). Programming sequences of hand postures. Journal of Motor Behavior, 19, 77–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann, J. & Koch, I. (1997). Stimulus-response compatibility and sequential learning in the serial reaction time task. Psychological Research, 60, 87–97.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A. & Stöcker, C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in serial reaction time tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 470–482.

    PubMed  CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1931). Goal attraction and directing ideas conceived as habit phenomena. Psychological Review, 38, 487–506.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Hulstijn, W. & van Galen G. P. (1983). Programming in handwriting: Reaction time and movement time as a function of sequence length. Acta Psychologica, 54, 23–49.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Hulstijn, W. & van Galen, G. P. (1988). Levels of motor programming in writing familiar and unfamiliar symbols. In A. M. Colley & J. K. Beech (eds.), Cognition and action in skilled behavior(pp. 65–85). Amsterdam: North-Holland/Elsevier.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Inhoff, A. W. (1986). Preparing sequences of saccades under choice reaction conditions: Effects of sequence length and context. Acta Psychologica, 61, 211–228.

    PubMed  CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: Dover.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Keele, S. W. & Posner, M. I. (1968). Processing of visual feedback in rapid movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 155–158.

    PubMed  CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Klapp, S. T. & Wyatt, E. P. (1976). Motor programming within a sequence of resposes. Journal of Motor Behavior, 8, 19–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kornbrot, D. E. (1989). Organisation of keying skills: The effect of motor complexity and number of units. Acta Psychologica, 70, 19–41.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L.A. Jeffress (ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior(pp. 112–146). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G. (1987). The organization of perception and action. Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokio: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Galanter, E. & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S. (1986). Programming of complex sequences: Evidence from the timing of rapid speech and other productions. In H. Heuer & C. Fromm (eds.), Generation and modulation of action patterns(pp. 72–86). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokio: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Münsterberg, H. (1889). Beiträge zur experimentellen Psychologie, Heft 1. Freiburg: Mohr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nattkemper, D. & Prinz, W. (1997). Stimulus and response anticipation in a serial reaction task. Psychological Research, 60, 98–112.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Portier, S. J. & van Galen, G. P. (1992). Immediate vs. postponed visual feedback in practising a handwriting task. Human Movement Science, 11, 563–592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Potter, J. M. (1980). What was the matter with Dr. Spooner? In V. A. Fromkin (ed.), Errors in linguistic performance(pp. 13–34). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Povel, D. & Collard, R. (1982). Structural factors in patterned finger tapping. Acta Psychologica, 52, 107–123.

    PubMed  CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A. (1980). Human movement initiation: Specification of arm, direction, and extent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 444–474.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A. (1991). Human motor control. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Hindorff, V. & Munro, E. M. (1987). Scheduling and programming of rapid finger sequences: Tests and elaborations of the hierarchical editor model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 193–203.

    PubMed  CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Inhoff, A. W. & Gordon, A. M. (1984). Choosing between movement sequences: A hierarchical editor model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 372–393.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Kenny, S. B. & Derr, M. A. (1983). Hierarchical control of rapid movement sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 86–102.

    PubMed  CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. & Norman, D. A. (1982). Simulating a skilled typist: A study of skilled cognitive-motor performance. Cognitive Science, 6, 1–36.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, G. (1933). Über Rückfälligkeit bei Umgewöhnung: 2. Über Handlungsganzheiten und ihre Bedeutung für die Rückfälligkeit. Psychologische Forschung, 18, 143–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semjen, A. & Garcia-Colera, A. (1986). Planning and timing of finger-tapping sequences with a stressed element. Journal of Motor Behavior, 18, 287–322.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Semjen, A. & Gottsdanker, R. (1991). Structural facilitation of movement sequence planning. In J. Requin & G. E. Stelmach (eds.), Tutorials in motor neuroscience(pp. 397–412). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Servan-Schreiber, E. & Anderson, J. R. (1990). Chunking as a mechanism of implicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 592–608.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Shaffer, L. H. (1975). Control processes in typing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27A, 419–432.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stelmach, G. E., Mullins, P. A. & Teulings H. (1984). Motor programming and temporal patterns in handwriting. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 144–157.

    PubMed  CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S., Monsell, S., Knoll, R. L. & Wright, C. E. (1978). The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences: Comparisons of speech and typewriting. In G. E. Stelmach (ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning(pp. 117–152). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terzuolo, C. A. & Viviani, P. (1980). Determinants and characteristics of motor patterns used for typing. Neuroscience, 5, 1085–1103.

    PubMed  CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Teulings, H., Mullins, P. A. & Stelmach, G. E. (1986). The elementary units of programming in handwriting. In G. P. van Galen, H. S. R. Kao & R. Hoosain (eds.), Graphonomics: Contemporary research in handwriting(pp. 21–32). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Wickelgren, W. A. (1969). Context-sensitive coding, associative memory, and serial order in (speech) behavior. Psychological Review, 76, 1–15.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zießler, M., Hänel, K. & Hoffmann, J. (1988). Die Programmierung struktureller Eigenschaften von Bewegungsfolgen. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 196, 371–388.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

Copyright information

© 2011 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Hommel, B., Nattkemper, D. (2011). Planung von Handlungssequenzen. In: Handlungspsychologie. Springer-Lehrbuch. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-12858-5_7

Download citation